first 3 articles

Journal of Fundamentals

of Mental Health

Mashhad University

of Medical Sciences

Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

Research Center

Original Article

Neuropsychological characteristics and theory of mind in ADHD

and normal students

Kobra Abazari1*

; Mohammad Reza Mahdavi2

; Abolfazl Darvishi3

1 Ph.D. in psychology of children with special needs, Instructor of Farhangian University 2 M.A. student of clinical psychology , Azad university of Eslamshahr, Iran

3 B.A in general psychology, Payam-e-Noor University, Baharestan, Iran

Abstract

Introduction: Comprehensive neuropsychological assessment considers symptoms in an individual, based on the

understanding of structure and function of the brain, and can lead to a better understanding of the symptoms, and

eventually more effective treatment recommendations. The aim of this research was to study and compare the

neuropsychological character and theory of mind as well as the correlation between them in ADHD and normal children.

Materials and Methods: This was a casual-comparative study. 25 ADHD children were selected through convenience

sampling and 25 normal children were selected through cluster sampling. Finally, the Connors Neuropsychological

Inventory and Theory of Mind Test were applied to assess the participants. The data were analyzed through multivariate,

t test, and Pearson correlation.

Results: Our findings showed that there is a significant difference between these two groups in terms of the theory of

mind and the three subscales of the neuropsychological inventory (attention problem (P=0.001), executive function

(P=0.0001), and reading function (P=0.027). ADHD children had lower efficiency than normal children did in theory of

mind (P=0.009) and the three subscales of the neuropsychological inventory. However, there was no correlation between

neuropsychological characteristics and theory of mind in ADHD and normal children.

Conclusion: It can be concluded that neuropsychological tests can be used as a complement to behavioral tests for

diagnostic assessment of ADHD children. In addition, it was observed that ADHD children require rehabilitations

program focused on academic performance, attention problems, and reading performance, as well as theory of mind.

Hence, the necessary measures should be taken for these children.

Keywords: Attention deficit hyperactive disorder, Neuropsychologic, Theory of mind

Please cite this paper as:

Abazari K, Mahdavi MR, Darvishi A. Neuropsychological characteristics and theory of mind in ADHD and normal students. Journal

of Fundamentals of Mental Health 2017 Jan-Feb; 19(1): 22-9.

Introduction

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a

neuro-developmental disorder, is characterized by

three main features including attention deficit,

hyperactivity and impulsivity, which it affects on 3

to 7 percent of children (1). Some of the evidence

declares that the physical abnormalities are the

reason to prevalence of the attention deficithyperactivity behavior (2,3), As a result, the disorder

is conceptualized as Neurodevelopment disorder (4-

8).

Partial damage to the brain, which has mentioned as

the cause of hyperactivity, is not recognizable

because of the fallibility of neurological tests (9).

The studies based on Neuroimaging have raised the

conflict between the sub-cortical and thalamocortical processes in frontal networks. In recent

decades, influenced by these findings, the

neuropsychological pattern related to attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was proposed by

neuropsychology. The cognitive deficits, exclusive

damage in attention and executive function are the

main hypothesis proposed in the scope of this

disorder. The children suffering ADHD gain the

lower scores and exhibit a weaker performance in

multiple tasks such as vigilance, sustained attention

and motor inhibition, executive function, verbal

learning and memory (10-15). It is reported that the

deficit pattern is similar to the findings obtained

from the frontal damaged individual and it has

considered as the basis of the frontal cortex damage

hypothesis or ADHD (10,16).

*Corresponding Author: Isfahan University, Isfahan, Iran

kabazari@yahoo.com

Received: Apr. 13, 2016

Accepted: Sep. 24, 2016

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 22

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 23

Diagnostic criteria, which are based on behavioral

symptoms, cannot sufficiently describe the nature of

some childhood disorders such as ADHD. In some

cases, these children exhibit the severe destruction

in operating function as well as deficits in motor

control and emotional regulation. The

comprehensive neuropsychological assessment,

which considers the available symptoms in

individuals and is based on the understanding the

structure and function of the brain, is led to greater

understanding of the symptoms, and eventually,

more effective treatment recommendations (17). In

addition, the available findings about the different

functional and structural development of ADHD

children’s brain indicates that several nervous

systems are involved in cognitive and motor

disorders as well as the emotional-behavioral

symptoms of the ADHD children. Thus, if the

comprehensive neuropsychological assessment does

not implement, some important condition, that there

is simultaneously in ADHD children, may not be

assessed (17). The new findings support the use of

neuropsychological tests for the differential

diagnosis between ADHD and normal individuals.

Although, the controversial findings have observed

in studies conducted on the normal and ADHD

individuals (13). For example, previous studies have

reported that there is no difference among the

normal and ADHD individual in case of

neuropsychological variable of visual attention (18).

Therefore, these studies show the importance of

neuropsychological tests to assess and clarify the

strengths and weaknesses of the attention in

individual, especially the recognition, and ultimately

to identify the ADHD individuals. Furthermore, the

studies suggest that the neuropsychological test to

diagnose the ADHD in individual is more important

than the use of the only measurement of individual

based on the evaluation tools such as DSM criteria

(19, 20). In addition, it is possible that the deficit in

executive function and neurological problems of

ADHD children such as attention and memory be

due to the deficit of their theory of mind (21).

Theory of mind, or the ability to ascribe the mental

states to oneself and other, is considered as an

outstanding achievement in human development.

Theory of mind allows us to consider the thoughts

and feelings of others beyond our own first-person

perspective. Also, this feature allows us to prepare

for participation in complex social interactions, yet

convenient (22). Previous studies have indicated that

ADHD children are lower than normal children in

theory of mind are (23-28).

However, the development of theory of mind is

initiated by rudimentary skills including attention,

the use of mental condition and pretend play in the

early stages of children's life. The recent studies

have suggested a close link between executive

functions and theory of mind. The researchers claim

that the carrying out the task of theory of mind

significantly requires the executive functions

(9,23,29-31). The results imply that there is the

fundamental involvement of executive functions in

the development of theory of mind. This view is

consistent with the findings of other researchers

(22,24).

Generally, ADHD and the disorders associated

with ADHD lead to difficulty of the definition,

assessment and treatment of this disorder.

Therefore, it is important to consider the wide range

of factors and conditions that cause the inattention,

hyperactivity, impulsivity (32). In addition, there are

some contradictions in the neuropsychological

features of the ADHD individual. Thus, this study

is necessary to detect the disorder in school-age

children, considering the neuropsychological

features and theory of mind of the ADHD children

compared with normal children. Hence, the aim of

this study was to evaluate the neuropsychological

features and theory of mind in children with ADHD

compared with normal children. In addition, this

study was to clarify whether the neurological

problems are related to the theory of mind of ADHD

children.

Materials and Methods

This study, based on its purpose and application, is

a casual-comparative, which is one of the

descriptive (non-experimental) research methods.

The statistical population of this study had been

consisted of the normal and hyperactive boys of

Eslamshahr city. The research was conducted with

the permission of the Administration of Education

of Eslamshahr city.

The sample of ADHD children was selected from

available ADHD boys (age group of 7-12 year old)

in the Razi Counseling Center of Eslamshahr. All

of the test subjects have expressed their contest

towards the participation in the test. It has also

emphasized that the information will be

confidential. The CSI4 test (parent's form) was

implemented to complete the diagnosis of ADHD in

children who were previously diagnosed as the

hyperactive by a psychiatrist. Finally, a number of

37 children have selected as the initial samples. The

inclusion criteria of the subjects in this study were

the normal intelligence range obtained through the

Stanford-Binet intelligence test that was conducted

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 24

on 37 hyperactive children. A number of seven

individual had average intelligence lesser than 90

and they excluded from the study due to this

problem. Finally, the children with IQ range of 90-

120 were included in this study. Of other exclusion

criteria, in addition to IQ 90, was the presence of

the symptoms of autism as well as other associated

disorders, i.e., anxiety, depression and epilepsy and

seizure problems. The observation of the child and

parents’ interview indicated that two children had

the autism symptoms, therefore they has excluded

from the study. There were no epilepsy symptoms in

37 selected children but three of them had anxiety

disorders. Eventually, 25 boys in the age group of 7-

12 years old were included into the study. In order

to assess the associated disorders (autism, anxiety

and depression), CSI4 questionnaire was used. The

children were referred to a psychiatrist for the

implementation of the EEG to measure the

symptoms of children with epilepsy and seizure in

addition to background checks and interviews with

parents and family of the child.

A list of boys’ ordinary schools at the elementary

level at Eslamshahr city were provided to achieve

the groups of children that can provide suitable

statistical analysis, i.e., age and gender appropriate

to the requirements of this study. Then, the boy’s

school of Shohdayeh 17 Shahrivar was selected by

the cluster sampling. After achieving the permission

to attend and research in that school, the list of

students, who had not notable disorder based on the

verification of school counselor, was given and we

randomly selected 25 students to participate in the

study. The questionnaire of ADHD and associated

disorders in this group was implemented based on

CSI4 test (the parent form). According the results of

this questionnaire, the number of 2 children had the

anxiety symptoms and one of children had the

depression symptoms. These three children were

excluded from study, three other normal students

were randomly replaced, and CSI4 test were

performed. Since the teachers and academic and

administrative staff confirmed that these students

have good learning situation, the Intelligence Test

was not conducted. The results indicated that there

was no disorder in these students. Finally, the

studied students along with their parents responded

to the questions of neurological questionnaire and

theory of mind tests.

Research instruments

In this study, four tests were utilized that include:

- Theory of Mind Test (TOM TEST): The main

form of theory of mind test has been designed in

order to assess the theory of mind in normal children

and children with pervasive developmental disorders

at the ages of 5 to 12 years. It provides the

information about the extent of social perception,

sensitivity and insight of children. In addition, it

clarifies the rate and degree to which the children

are able to accept the feelings and thoughts of others

(33). The reliability of the test by Cronbach's alpha

was obtained to be 0.92, 0.84, 0.86 and 0.85 for the

total scale, first scale, second scale and third scale,

respectively (33).

Qmrany et al. created some changes in this test.

They reduced the number of questions of the test

from 72 to 38 and used the Persian name instead of

the foreign names. Then, they measured the validity

and reliability of the test on a number of normal and

educable mental retarded (mild) students at Shiraz.

The content validity method, simultaneous validity

and correlation of subscale with total score were

used to evaluate the validity of the test.

Simultaneous validity was estimated to be 89%

through the correlation of test with the Dollhouse

task, which it was significant at the level of 0.01.

The correlation coefficient of the subtests with the

total scores was significant in all case and it was in

range of 0.92 to 0.98. Test-retest reliability was

between 70% and 96% and the entire coefficient

was significant at the level of 0.01. Internal stability

of the test (using Cronbach's alpha) for the whole

test and each of subtests was calculated to be 86%,

72%, 80% and 81%, respectively.

This test has designed based on a multidimensional and evolutionary perspective of the

theory of mind (33) and it is able to assess the

greater range of age groups and more sophisticated

and complex levels of theory of mind rather than the

older tests such as dollhouse task, boxes of Smartiz,

etc. The main form of the test consists of 78

questions and 3 subtests, which are as follows:

 First subscale: Preliminary theory of

mind, i.e., the first level of theory of mind

or recognition of emotions and pretend,

consisting of 20 questions.

 Second subscale: Initial statements of an

actual theory of mind, i.e., the second level

of theory of mind or initial false belief and

understanding of false belief, consisting of

13 questions.

 Third subscale: Advanced concepts of

theory of mind, i.e, the third level of

theory of mind or secondary false belief

and understanding of joke, consisting of

five questions.

Participants can receive scores between 0 and 20 in

first subtest; scores between 0 and 13 in second

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 25

subscale; scores between 0 and 5 in the third

subscale; and scores between 0 and 38 in the total

score. The sum of the scores obtained from above

three subscales gives an overall score for theory of

mind. Higher scores indicate that the child has

achieved a higher level of theory of mind (33).

- Connors Neuropsychological Inventory (Parents

Form): Connors designed this test to assess the

neuro-cognitive problems with children in age range

of 5-12 years. This test assesses the problems of

attention, sensory-motor function, language,

executive functions, memory, learning and cognition

in four spectrums (unobserved to severe). Jadidi and

Abedi have translated and standardized this

questionnaire and they have obtained its validity

using the factor analysis method. They have

reported that this tool has appropriate validity. The

reliability of this tool using Cronbach's Alpha has

reported to be 0.72. The reliability of subscales was

also determined using Cronbach's Alpha and it was

observed that the reliability of various variables

including executive function, attention, sensorymotor function, language and memory & learning

was 0.71, 0.74, 0.78, 0.69 and 0.79, respectively

(35).

- The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test: AFrooz and

Kamkari developed the fifth Edition of StanfordBinet in 2008. It is utilized to test IQ for the age

range of 2-90 years old and it can be used in the

areas of identification, diagnosis and placement of

individuals in special education programs. The

profile has concentrated to identify the detailed

performance of the individuals in the 10 subtests

with emphasis on two verbal and non-verbal areas,

which it is consisted of 10 subtests according to 5-

intelligence factor. These 10 subtests in verbal and

nonverbal scale include fluid reasoning, knowledge,

quantitative reasoning, visual-spatial processing and

working memory. In addition, the correlation

between the two areas of non-verbal and verbal

obtained to be between 0.94 and 0.97. The validity

coefficients of this test were extracting to be

between 0.84 and 0.89 among the 10 subscales this

intelligence test. The coefficients calculated for the

tool represents that the tool has high credit for

subtest and combined scores (36). In Iran, The

validity of this test along with Wechsler for the

verbal IQ, non-verbal IQ, and general IQ was 0.58,

0.59 and 0.66, respectively (37).

- Child Symptom Inventory-4 (CSI4): The last

edition of this questionnaire has two forms for

parents and teachers that it shows its suitable

effectiveness compared to other measures and

practices. Furthermore, the diagnostic criteria of this

test are Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) of

Mental Disorders criteria and its terms and phrases

are the simple and understandable. Two have

designed for scoring methods for Child Symptom

Inventory: Scoring methods based on cut off point

screening and the scoring method based on the

severity of symptoms. In the screening method, the

method of scoring is obtained by sum of the number

of phrases that which are responded by the options

"sometimes" or "often". If the answer of the

questions of the test were the "never" or "rarely", the

"zero" is given to reply to those questions, while the

score of "1" is given to the answers of "sometimes"

or "more often". In scoring by severity of symptoms

method, the options of "never", "rarely",

"sometimes", "more often" is scored with the codes

of "0", "1", "2" and "3", respectively and the sum of

the obtained scores give the symptom severity.

Sprafkin and Gadow et al. (2001) have investigated

the reliability and validity of the Child Symptom

Inventory (CSI_4) questionnaire. The results of this

study have showed that there was satisfactory

internal consistency reliability, test-retest reliability

as well as stability during a period of 4 years. The

CSI-4 scale had convergent validity with the Child

Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and children diagnostic

interview. In addition, the CSI-4 scale had had

divergent validity with parents modified form

(DICA-P) (38). Ismaili & Alipour obtained the

reliability and validity of this test for children at

Tehran at 2002. The validity of this test for

abnormalities was achieved as follows: ADHD 60%,

ADHD predominantly inattentive type (ADHD-PI)

0.53%, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type

0.69%, Only hyperactivity 0.70, depression 0.56,

Anxiety 0.62, autism 0.58 (39).

Results

The statistical analysis results showed that 11 years

old children have the highest frequency in this

study. Generally, the mean age of the study was

9.26 years. In addition, the 11-year-old children had

the highest frequency in the normal group of

children in that the mean age of the normal group

was 9.73 years and its variance is equal with 3.21.

The standard deviation of this group is 1.79. Among

the ADHD group, 11-year-old children had also the

highest frequency. The mean of age and its variance

are 8.8 and 4.45, respectively. The standard

deviation (SD) of this group was calculated to be

2.11.

Table1 shows the descriptive scores of hyperactive

and normal children in neurological variables.

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 26

Table 1. Mean and SD of the subscales of Connors

test among subjects

Subscales Status Mean SD

1 Attention Problems Normal 16.8 10.15

Hyperactive 29.06 8.15

2 Sensori-motor Function Normal 1.86 3.06

Hyperactive 5.53 14.40

3 Language Functions Normal 3.2 5.28

Hyperactive 5.6 5.81

4 Learning and Memory

Function

Normal 7.13 9.47

Hyperactive 14.06 11.84

5 Executive Functions Normal 6.86 6.55

Hyperactive 18.86 9.63

6 Ability and Speed of

Cognitive Processing

Normal 3.4 4.67

Hyperactive 6.33 6.27

7 Visual and Spatial

Performance

Normal 1.26 2.15

Hyperactive 2.66 3.06

8 Academic Performance

(Reading)

Normal 4.33 6.22

Hyperactive 10.93 9.01

9 Academic Performance

(Writing )

Normal 9.2 8.87

Hyperactive 13.8 15.21

10 Academic

Performance(Math)

Normal 5.2 7.07

Hyperactive 5.66 5.97

The Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)

test was used to compare the differences between

the scores of ADHD and normal individual obtained

from the subscales of Connors neuropsychological

test and the results were represented following table

(Table 2).

Table 2. Differences between hyperactive and

normal groups of children in neuropsychological

variables

Variables

Degrees of

freedom (df)

Mean of

square

F ratio

Significance

level

Efficiency

coefficient

Attention Problems 1 1128.533 13.303 0.001 0.322

Sensorimotor Function 1 100.833 0.93 0.343 0.032

Language Functions 1 43.200 1.4 0.247 0.048

Learning and Memory

Function

1 360.533 3.136 0.087 0.101

Executive Functions 1 1080 15.904 0.0001 0.362

Ability and Speed of

Cognitive Processing

1 64.533 2.109 0.158 0.070

Visual and Spatial

Performance

1 14.7 2.097 0.159 0.070

Academic Performance

(Reading)

1 326.7 5.444 0.027 0.163

Academic Performance

(Writing )

1 158.7 1.023 0.321 0.035

Academic Performance

(Math)

1 1.633 0.038 0.847 0.001

Multivariate analysis (MANOVA) was used to

show the difference between the normal and ADHD

groups of students; Wilks's Lambda with significant

level of 0.0001 represents the difference between

normal and ADHD children in neuropsychological

features.

As can be seen in Table 2, ADHD children had

more problems in three subscale of children's

Connors neuropsychological test including

“attention problems”, “executive functions” and

“academic performance in reading" than normal

children.

In the theory of mind test, the mean of normal

children (SD=3.08) was obtained to be 30.6 while it

was 25.53 for the ADHD children (SD=6.03). In

order to understand the significant differences

between the two groups of children in test of theory

of mind, the independent t-test was used and the

results indicated that there is significant difference

between two groups for the theory of mind test

(t=2.77, df=28 and significant level=0.009). Based

on this result, the group of ADHD children has

lower score in theory of mind than normal children.

In addition, the Pearson correlation test was

applied to measure the correlation level of the

Connors neuropsychological tests and theory of

mind for both of normal and ADHD children. The

results showed that the correlation between theory

of mind and the scores obtained from Connors

neuropsychological test for normal children is as r=-

0.338, p=0.228, there was no significant correlation

at the level of 0.05. The measurement of correlation

between theory of mind and the scores obtained

from Connors neuropsychological test was also

conducted for ADHD children and the results was

r= -0.105, p= 0.701; there was no significant

correlation at the level of 0.05

Discussion

This study aimed to evaluate the neurological

characteristics and theory of mind in ADHD and

normal children at the age of 7-12 years. The results

indicate that the ADHD children were significantly

different from normal children solely in the scale of

attention, executive functions and reading among

the 10 neurological subscales; it means that they had

lower performance in these three scales. The results

of the study are in accordance with results of some

other researchers. They believe that the ADHD and

healthy individual have statistically significant

difference in one or several tests of executive

functions. Approximately, 80 percent of ADHD

children have deficits, in at least one of the

components of executive function (10-14). Also,

Perner et al. express that ADHD children has not

good performance in activities that are often

required to selective attention and executive skills,

as well as sustained attention and executive skills, or

both (15).

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 27

As mentioned in the results, hyperactive children

are also weak in reading performance, in addition to

attention and executive functions, than normal

children. The researchers introduce the attention as

important factor for reading ability, which their

expression can be a proper justification to the

mentioned result. It has also reported that the

attention is important to convert the print into

speech. Therefore, the "attention" is necessary for

fluent reading (29). Some researchers concluded that

deficits of attention is caused the problems in a

person's reading (15, 19,20). Gatica has stated that

there is a relationship between the maintenance of

attention and reading without the loud. People with

good maintenance of attention have faster reading

ability than those with medium and low

maintenance. Therefore, the high ability to

maintenance the attention is leading to the

successful reading comprehension (21). Thus, in

present study, because the hyperactive children

received a low score on attention variable, they had

low scores in reading.

In the next part of this research, theory of mind

were compared between ADHD and normal

children and the results showed that ADHD children

have lower scores on theory of mind compared to

normal children. The results of present study are

confirmed by the previous studies (24-30).

Hughes and Ensor have been studied the theory of

mind among the primary school children with the

behavioral problems; In a sample of 130 children, it

was found that deficits in theory of mind task is a

strong predictor of behavior problems such as

conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder and

hyperactivity (20). Walker has been used the false

belief and showed that the theory of mind, after

controlling for age, can significantly predicted

aggression and disruptive behavior in boys (32).

Buitelaar et al. reported that ADHD children in the

false belief are considerably lower than the normal

children are (26).

Some researcher proposed that theory of mind

explains to parents the reason of externalizing

behavioral problems and understanding the mental

states and considering the opinions of others their

child (40). In addition, some theoreticians purpose

that failure in identification of emotions is observed

in children with externalizing problems (41).

Theoreticians of cognitive models believe that

various cognition such as beliefs, attributions and

expectations of yourself and others behavior are

very important in determination of feelings and

behavior; and individual, who have distorted,

inaccurate and inconsistent cognition about

themselves, others and their environment event,

exhibit problematic behavior and feelings.

Aggressive children are also in trouble in front of

peers and it is leading to show lesser empathy in

interaction with peers; and consequently, they will

rejected by their peer that it has significant

relationship with deficits of the of the theory of

mind task (42).

Of other results of the present study is that there is

no relationship among the neurological

characteristics and theory of mind in both ADHD

and normal children. Although, the studies, the

recently published papers about the relationship

between executive function and theory of mind,

have suggested a close link between executive

functions and theory of mind. The researchers claim

that the task of theory of mind is significantly

required to executive functions (9).

It is believed that their findings are statistically

shown the significant relationship between

inhibitory control and task of theory of mind. These

findings highlight the fundamental involvement of

executive functions in the development of theory of

mind. This view is consistent with the findings of

other researchers (30, 31). The studies show their

relationship between executive function in

preschool, which are externalized behavior

(attention, memory, behavioral inhibition, control or

impulsivity, self-regulation) and ideas of theory of

mind or emotions, emotion of their theory of mind

(29).

Of the limitations of this study was the use of only

one gender i.e. boys; thus, it is suggested that the

future studies conducted on both girls and boys and

the neuropsychological features to be investigate

and compared among both genders. Another

limitation is the use of only one tool to measure

neuropsychological features. It is recommended that

other tools be used to assess the neuropsychological

features of these types of children in future studies.

This test was only conducted in Eslamshahr and

children of this area, which it can be considered as

another limitation of this study. The next limitation

can be relatively small sample size, which it is due

to low availability of these children in centers. Since

the results of this study showed the poor

performance of ADHD children in educational

performance of reading, attention and executive

functions, and, since the results are consistent with

other studies conducted in other countries; therefore,

it is suggested that these three components be

applied in training and rehabilitation of ADHD

children. Furthermore, it is recommended that the

questionnaires and the neuropsychological

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 28

diagnostic tool be used to comprehensively assess

and detect the hyperactive children in addition to

interviews and observation methods,. It is also

recommended that the study be conducted for other

childhood psychiatric disorders and for children

with special needs such as learning disorders,

autism, and mental retardation. Another suggestion

is that this study be tested through other

neuropsychological questionnaires as well as other

psychiatric tools, which measure the

neuropsychological features. In addition, this study

can be carried out separately or comparatively in

various groups of the ADHD children (inattentive

type, hyperactive type, composition type). Finally, it

is recommended that this study be performed with

higher sample size and in other regions.

Conclusion

It can be concluded that neuropsychological tests

can be used as a complement to behavioral tests for

diagnostic assessment of ADHD children. In

addition, it was observed that ADHD children

require rehabilitations program focused on academic

performance, attention problems, and reading

performance, as well as theory of mind. Hence, the

necessary measures should be taken for these

children.

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted with the approval of

education administration of Eslamshahr city.

Hereby, we are grateful from the Razi Counseling

Center of Eslamshahr city to cooperate in this

research. In addition, there was no conflict of

interest between the authors.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC:

American Psychiatric Association. 4th ed; 2000.

2. Barry RJ, Johnstone SJ, Clarke AR. A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II.

Event related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol 2003; 114(2): 184-98.

3. Jonkman LM, Kenemans JL, Kemner C, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. Dipole source localization of event related

brain activity indicative of an early visual selective attention deficit in ADHD children. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;

115(7): 1537-49.

4. Castellanos EX, Glaser PEA, Gerhardt GA. Towards a neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:

Fractionating the phenotype. J Neurosci Methods 2006; 151(1): 1-4.

5. Rauch SL. Neuroimaging and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the 21st century: What to consider and how

to proceed. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57(11): 1261-2.

6. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: From common simple deficits to

multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry 2005; 57(11): 1231-8.

7. Swanson J, Castellanos FX, Murias M, LaHoste G, Kennedy J. Cognitive neuroscience of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998; 8(2): 263-71.

8. Taylor E. Developmental neuropsychopathology of attention deficit and impulsiveness. Dev Psychopathol 1999;

11(3): 607-28.

9. Strauss AA, Lehtinen LE. Psychopathology and education of the brain-injured child. New York: Grune and Stratton;

1947.

10. Sideman LJ, Lieberman J, Milberg SH, Norman D, Seired K. Effect of family history and comorbidity on the

neuropsychological performance of children with ADHD. Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995; 34(8): 1015-24.

11. Woods SP, Lovejoy DW, Ball JD. Neuropsychological characteristics of adults with ADHD: a comprehensive

review of initial studies. Clin Neuropsychol 2002; 16: 12-34.

12. Boonstra AM, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J, Buitelaar J. Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review.

Psychol Med 2005; 35: 1097-108.

13. Dobson-Patterson R, O’Gorman J, Chan R, Shum D. ADHD subtypes and neuropsychological performance in an

adult sample. Res Dev Disabil 2016; 55: 55-63.

14. Pennington B. Toward a new neuropsychological model of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Subtypes

and multiple deficits. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 7: 1221-3.

15. Perner J, Lang B, Kilo D. Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Dev

2002; 73: 752-67.

16. Teeter PA, Wender EH. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder on adolescence. J Dev Behav 1998; 30: 5.

17. Pitcher TM, Piek JP, Hay DA. Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol 2003; 45:

525-35.

18. Laasonen M, Salomaa J, Cousineau D, Leppämäki S, Tani P, Hokkanen L, et al. Project DyAdd: Visual attention in

adult dyslexia and ADHD. Brain Cogn 2012; 80: 311-27.

19. Saeidi M, Bafandeh H, Noorazar GH. [Comparison of theory of mind in ADHD and control group. Proceeding of 5th

International Congress of Child Psychiatry. Tehran: Iran, 2012. (Persian)

THEORY OF MIND AND ADHD ABAZARI, MAHDAVI, DARVISHI

Fundamentals of Mental Health, 2017 Jan-Feb http://jfmh.mums.ac.ir 29

20. Hughes C, Ensor R. Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Dev Psychol

2007; 43: 1447-59.

21. Gatica F, Redin C, Abaitua Ch. Emotional and cognitive profile of adolescents with ADHD: Effects of learning

mediated interaction. Procedia Soc Behav Sci 2013; 84: 1704-11.

22. Hughes C, Dunn J, White A. Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction

in hard-to-manage preschoolers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39: 981-94.

23. Grosbois N, Houssa N, Mazzone M. How could Theory of Mind contribute to the differentiation of social

adjustment profiles of children with externalizing behavior disorders and children with intellectual disabilities? Res Dev

Disabil 2013; 34: 2642-60.

24. Mize J, Pettit GS. Social information-processing and the development of conduct problems in children and

adolescents: Looking beneath the surface. In: Sharp C, Fonagy P, Goodyer I. (editors). Social cognition and

developmental psychopathology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008: 141-74.

25. Pettit GS, Dodge KA, Brown MM. Early family experience, social problem solving patterns, and children’s social

competence. Child Dev 1998; 59: 107-20.

26. Buitelaar JK, Van Deer Wees M, Swaab-Barneveld H, Jan Van Deer Gaag R. Theory of mind and emotionrecognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children. Dev Psychopathol

1999; 11: 39-58.

27. Hughes C, Leekam S. What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review and reflections and

new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Soc Dev 2004; 13: 590-619.

28. Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggress Behav 2006; 32:

540-50.

29. Perugini EM, Harvey EA, Lovejoy DW, Sandstrom K, Webb AH. The predictive power of combined

neuropsychological measures for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Child Neuropsychol 2000; 6: 101-

14.

30. Ruff ME. Attention deficit disorder and stimulant use: An epidemic of modernity. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2005; 44(7):

557-63.

31. Roth RW, Saykin AJ. Executive dysfunction in attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: Cognitive and neuromaging

finding. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2004; 27(1): 83-96.

32. Walker S. Gender differences in the relationship between young children's peer-related social competence and

individual differences in theory of mind. Genet Psychol 2005; 166(3): 297-312.

33. Muris P, Stresemann P, Messters C, Marckelbach H, Horselenberg R, Van Den-Hogen T, et al. The tom test: A new

instrument for assessing theory of mind in normal children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev

Disord 1999; 29(1): 67-80.

34. Qmrany A, Alborzi Sh, Khayer M. [Study of validity and reliability of theory of mind test in a group of mental

retardation and normal students in Shiraz]. Journal of psychology 2006; 10(38): 181-99. (Persian)

35. Jadidi M, Abedi A. [Accommodation and normalization of Connor neuropsychology inventory in children with 5-12

years old in Isfahan]. Isfahan, Iran: University of Isfahan. (Persian)

36. Farid F, Kamkari K, Safarnya M, Afrooz S. [Comparison of validity of Tehran-new IQ Inventory, Stanford-Binet

inventory and fourth version of Wechsler intelligence scale among children with learning disabilities. Journal of

learning disability 2013; 4: 70-83. (Persian)

37. Shiri Aminloo M , Kamkary K, Shokrzadeh S. [The concurrent validity of the new version of the Tehran-StanfordBinet Intelligence Scale with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised]. Exceptional education 2013; 7: 50-

61. (Persian)

38. Sprafkin J, Gadow KD, Salisbury H, Schneider J, Long J. Further evidence of reliability and validity of the child

symptom Inventory-4: parent checklist in clinically referred boys. Department of psychiatry, state university of New

York, NY; 2002: 11794-879.

39. Alipour A, Mohammad Esmail E. [Credit check and determine cut-off points disorder Child Symptom Inventoryyear-old CSI-4 on elementary and secondary school students 6-14 year in Tehran]. Tehran: Exceptional Children

Research Institute; 2003. (Persian)

40. Capage L, Watson AC. Individual differences in theory of mind, aggressive behavior, and social skills in young

children. Early Educ Dev 2001; 12: 613-28.

41. Marsh AA, Blair RJR. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neurosci

Biobehav Rev 2008; 32: 454-65.

42. Razavieh A, Latifian M, Arefi M. The role of theory of mind and empathy in predicting students overt and sociable

aggressive behavior. Journal of psychological study 2007; 2(3-4): 25-38.

Copyright of Journal of Fundamentals of Mental Health is the property of Mashhad

University of Medical Sciences, Center Office of Fundamentals of Mental Health and its

content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the

copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email

articles for individual use.

 

Introduction

T

HERE IS relatively high prevalence of

attentional difficulties amongst children

and young people in the UK, with Ford,

Goodman and Meltzer (2003) reporting that

3.62 per cent of boys and 0.85 per cent of

girls in the UK have Attention/Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), with many cases

persisting into adulthood (Lara et al., 2009).

Attentional difficulties, such as ADHD, are

also associated with peer rejection and low

self-esteem (Klassen, Miller & Fine, 2004), as

well as social anxiety and stress (Elkins et al.,

2011). Research suggests that impoverished

environmental input within the classroom

can prevent primary-aged pupils from realising their underlying attentional potential

(see Ruff & Rothbart, 1996; Schweizer, Moosbrugger & Goldhammer, 2005), which may

then compromise their cognitive development and academic progress (see Breslau et

al., 2010; Steele et al., 2012).

A neuropsychological model of attention

Attention ‘is the process by which certain information is selected for further processing and other

information is discarded’ (Ward, 2006, p.130),

and is characterised by its limited capacity

and responsivity to both sensory (auditory

and visual) and semantic stimulus characteristics (Lezak, Howieson & Loring, 2004,

p.34).

An early neuropsychological model of

attention by Luria (1973) postulated two

parallel attentional systems: the reflexive

attentional system (RAS) and the volitional

attentional system (VAS). The RAS responds

to biologically meaningful environmental

stimuli, whilst the VAS, characterised by individuals’ interpretations of environmental

stimuli, requires higher-order cognition to

work efficiently. It is widely accepted that

attentional skills improve with age (Cooley &

Morris, 1990), with Luria (1973) stating that

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 51

© The British Psychological Society, 2016

Measuring the effectiveness of a

mindfulness-based intervention for

children’s attentional functioning

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

Aim: This study sought to evaluate the impact of a six-hour mindfulness programme (Paws .b) on

mainstream primary school aged pupils’ suppressing and sustaining attention skills.

Method: A randomised control trial (RCT) design with quasi-experimental intervention cross-lag was used

with two classes of 8- to 9-year-olds. Pupils and class teachers were randomly assigned to the experimental

or waitlist control group, with each receiving the intervention over six weeks. Teacher-reported and

standardised attention measures were gathered at pre- and post-intervention, and at follow-up.

Findings: Within-condition comparisons revealed several significant pre- vs. post-intervention effects, the

majority of which were maintained at follow-up. Between-condition comparisons revealed some significant

partial condition × time-point interactions.

Limitations: Quasi-experimental research within ecologically valid real-world settings is prone to the

influence of confounding variables. In this case, a change of class teacher within the waitlist control group

may have had a marked impact on the findings.

Conclusions: The present study offers tentative evidence of the effectiveness of Paws .b in improving

attentional functioning in primary-aged children. Findings are discussed relative to mindfulness and

attention literature, and further implications for future research are outlined.

Keywords: mindfulness; attention; classroom intervention; cross-lag; quasi-experiment.

the RAS is present from early infancy, with

the VAS emerging later in childhood. Ruff

and Rothbart (1996) also identified an interactional process, whereby neural substrates

dictate potential attentional development

throughout infancy and childhood, with environmental input determining the extent to

which underlying potential is realised. Therefore, the classroom potentially serves as the

ideal environment in which to develop

pupils’ attention, particularly as ‘schoolchildren and adolescents of all levels of intelligence

have difficulties in attention’ (Das, 2002, p.241).

Strauss et al. (2000) stated that a

neuropsychological approach to attention is

characterised by separating the parameters

of attention into independent components,

relative to performance on various neuropsychological tasks. There are five attentional

components: suppressing attention; sustaining attention; focusing attention; shifting

attention; and divided attention (see

Anderson et al., 2002; Mirsky et al., 1991;

Stuss et al., 1995). Working definitions of

each are shown in Table 1 below.

Thomas (2013) asked Special Educational Needs Co-ordinators (SENCOs) about

which attentional components were most

commonly used and required by mainstream

primary-aged pupils. Findings indicated that

SENCOs viewed sustaining attention (Mirsky

et al., 1991) and supressing attention (Stuss

et al., 1995) as most important. This

provided a rationale for the present study’s

focus on suppressing and sustaining attention skills within the classroom. Additionally,

MacKay (2005) offers a rationale for the use

of a neuropsychological approach within an

educational setting, in that ‘neuropsychology

should not be seen as a specialist “bolt-on” area

within educational psychology, but rather as part

of the essential knowledge and practice base

required for the effective delivery of EP services’

(p.7).

Mindfulness-based attention state training

Petersen and Posner (2012) suggest there

are two broad methods of attentional

improvement: attention training (practice

using a particular attentional network) and

attention state training (practice using forms

of meditation to alter the neural state in a

way that improves attention). Tang and

Posner (2009) highlight that the focus of

attention training is to use cognitive tasks to

practise using particular attentional

networks, whereas the focus of attention

state training is for experiences, such as

mindfulness, to effect the mind and improve

performance. Given the prominence of

experiential learning within the primary

National Curriculum (Department for

Education [DfE], 2013), it was felt that attention state training would be a more appropriate focus for the present study.

Mindfulness has its origins in many

cultural, contemplative, and philosophical

traditions, particularly Buddhism (KabatZinn, 2000), and is defined as ‘the awareness

that emerges through paying attention on purpose,

52 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

Table 1: Working definitions of the five attentional components.

Attentional component Definition

Suppressing attention The ability to inhibit prepotent responses, or to supress impulsive

responses

Sustaining attention The capacity to maintain focus and alertness over time, or vigilance

Focusing attention The ability to select target information from an array for enhanced

processing

Shifting attention The ability to change attentive focus in a flexible and adaptive manner

Divided attention When two or more tasks have to be carried out at the same time

in the present moment and non-judgementally

(Kabat-Zinn, 2003, p.45). Typically, mindfulness practices include breathing, body scan,

movement, and listening exercises whilst

ensuring that one supresses their attention

towards intrusive thoughts/judgements/

mind wanderings (Williams & Penman,

2011). Whilst the Melbourne Academic

Mindfulness Interest Group (MAMIG, 2006)

proposes that mindfulness is a metacognitive

skill, others assert that it also involves

suppressing and sustaining attention (see

Bishop et al., 2004; Lutz et al., 2008).

Jones (2011) suggests mindfulness has as

much of a place within the education system

as exams, as it prepares pupils for life outside

of school. Becuase of its secular nature, mindfulness should not clash with pupils’ religious

or cultural beliefs and thus should be implementable within a multicultural classroom

(Lubelska, 2012). Goodman (2005) and

Kabat-Zinn (1990) suggest that children’s

openness to experience, readiness to learn,

and creativity make them more likely to

benefit from mindfulness than adults.

There are a growing number of studies

citing the positive effects of mindfulness on

children’s and adolescents’ attentional functioning (Felver et al., 2014; Semple et al.,

2010; van der Oord, Bögels & Peijnenburg,

2012). However, Greenberg and Harris

(2012) noted that few trials with children

have been conducted with mainstream

populations and that those which exist have

inherent problems with design, sample size

and measurement.

One exception is a study by Napoli,

Krech and Holley (2005) who evaluated the

impact of the Attention Academy

Programme, a manualised mindfulness

programme delivered by experienced mindfulness practitioners. The RCT design

involved 228 6- to 9-year-old mainstream

pupils randomly allocated to either an

experimental group or a control group.

Teachers completed pre- and post-intervention measures of pupils’ ADHD symptoms

using the ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher

Rating Scale (ACTeRS; Ullmann, Sleator &

Sprague, 1997), whilst pupils undertook

specific subtests from the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch; Manly et

al., 2001) pertaining to focusing and

sustaining attention. Findings demonstrated

that the experimental group had significantly lower teacher-rated ADHD symptoms

(r=0.49) and better focusing attention

performance (r=0.6) at post-intervention

compared to controls, although no significant difference was observed between both

groups at post-intervention in terms of

sustaining attention. However, limitations

included: the use of the ACTeRS, which is

tailored towards the identification of ADHD

symptoms and is not intended for use with

typically-developing children; the lack of

baseline between-condition comparisons;

and, the lack of follow-up assessment.

Mindfulness in schools

A debate exists as to whether mindfulness

teachers need to be established mindfulness

practitioners in order to teach mindfulness

effectively to children. Kostanski and Hassed

(2008) highlight this as a tradition but note

that robust scientific evidence for this is

limited. The MAMIG (2006) suggests that

mindfulness is traditionally taught in groups,

but with individual support, as this is costeffective; allows pupils to learn from one

another; and, allows greater pupil motivation due to peer support.

In terms of the intervention used in this

study, a well-established nine-week manualised mindfulness programme for teenagers

called ‘.b for teens’ (Mindfulness in Schools

Project, 2013b) was already being delivered

locally in secondary schools and sixth form

colleges. Following its success (Huppert &

Johnson, 2010; Kuyken et al., 2013), the

Mindfulness in Schools Project differentiated the .b for teens materials so that

primary-aged pupils could access the

curriculum content and mindfulness practices. This formed the basis of a new six-week

programme called ‘Paws .b’ (Mindfulness in

Schools Project, 2013a). The aim of the

present study was to evaluate the impact of

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 53

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

the Paws .b mindfulness programme on

mainstream primary-aged pupils’ suppressing and sustaining attention skills within the

UK context, and, in doing so, address the

following research question:

To what extent does the Paws .b mindfulness

programme lead to an improvement in mainstream primary school aged pupils’ suppressing

and sustaining attention skills?

Method

Sample

Convenience sampling was undertaken at a

mixed and ethnically diverse comprehensive

primary school in north-west England where

the first researcher was the link Educational

Psychologist (EP). Prior to the beginning of

the present study, the school had independently decided to introduce Paws .b.

The researchers made an ethical decision

to target Year 4 pupils (8- and 9-year-olds)

who are unlikely to be involved in preparation for Standard Assessment Tests (SATs)

taken in Year 6. Thirty Year 4 pupils were

recruited (mean age 8 years 10 months; 50

per cent female): 16 from class one (mean

age 8 years 9 months; 44 per cent females)

and 14 from class two (mean age 8 years

10 months; 57 per cent female). Both class

teachers were female. However, it should be

noted that the original waitlist control

teacher went on maternity leave half way

through the study and was replaced by

another female class teacher. The mindfulness teacher who delivered Paws .b to both

groups was an established mindfulness practitioner and had received training to deliver

Paws .b from the Mindfulness in Schools

Project.

Relative to the geographical location of

the school, all pupils had English as an additional language (EAL) with a predominance

of Urdu or Bengali as their first language.

However, through consultation with class

teachers and the mindfulness teacher, the

language demands of Paws .b were deemed

to be in line with the Year 4 National

Curriculum (DfE, 2013).

Design and procedure

A RCT design with a quasi-experimental

intervention cross-lag was utilised (see

Semple et al., 2010) with four data collection

time-points six to eight weeks apart: Baseline; Time 1; Time 2; and Time 3. Prior to

Baseline, the coin toss method was used to

randomly allocate class one to the experimental group, leaving class two in the waitlist

control group. The experimental group

received Paws .b between Baseline and Time1; the waitlist control group received Paws .b

between Time 1 and Time 2. Hence, the

intervention cross-lag took place at Time 1

when the mindfulness teacher cross-lagged

from delivering Paws .b to the experimental

group to delivering Paws .b to the waitlist

control group.

Within the experimental group, Time 2

and Time 3 acted as the eight-week and

14-week post-intervention follow-ups. For the

waitlist control group, Time 3 acted as the

six-week post-intervention follow-up. The

different follow-up periods reflected the

length of each school half-term. The relationship between the four time-points and

the intervention stages per condition are

represented in Table 2.

Materials

Paws .b consists of six one-hour lessons that

are delivered on a weekly basis. The materials consist of a Microsoft PowerPoint presentation and plan for each lesson. An

overview of Paws .b is represented in Table 3.

Implementation checks were carried out by

the first researcher throughout the two intervention periods (see Barnett, Hawkins &

Lentz Jr., 2011; Durlak & DuPre, 2008).

Measures and scoring

The teacher-reported attention measure

used within the present study was the

Attention Checklist (Das, 2002), which the

class teachers completed for all pupils at

each of the four time-points. It is a 12-item

questionnaire that pertains to observable

attention behaviours that pupils are likely to

exhibit within the classroom (e.g. ‘Does the

54 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

child daydream in class?’). Each attention

behaviours is rated using four descriptors:

‘Not at all’; ‘Just a little’; ‘Pretty much’; and,

‘Very much’; and is coded so that positive

attention behaviours receive higher scores.

The Attention Checklist was chosen due to it

being high in face validity relative to

assessing pupils’ suppressing and sustaining

attention skills.

The change of class teacher within the

waitlist control group took place immediately after Time 1. In an attempt to protect

the validity of the Attention Checklist

dataset, the second waitlist control teacher

was provided with the Attention Checklist

data which had been generated by the

original waitlist control teacher at Baseline

and Time 1. Through cross-standardisation,

the researchers hoped that the second waitlist control teacher would be able to establish

the range of attention behaviours typically

displayed by each pupil at Baseline and

Time 1 before carrying out their own ratings

at Time 2 and Time 3. Despite these efforts,

it is acknowledged that the change of class

teacher within the waitlist control group is a

confounding variable in the study.

The researcher-administered attention

measure used within the present study was

the NEPSY-II (Korkman, Kirk & Kemp,

2007) Inhibition subtest, which the first

researcher administered to all pupils at each

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 55

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

Table 2: Relationship between time-points and intervention stages per condition.

Condition Intervention stage

Baseline Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Experimental group Pre- Post- Follow-up 1 Follow-up 2 intervention intervention

Waitlist control group Pre pre- Pre- Post- Follow-up 1 intervention intervention intervention

Table 3: Overview of the Paws .b mindfulness programme.

Lesson number Description of content

1 l An introduction to the brain;

l A discussion of our ability to make choices;

l A breath counting mindfulness exercise.

2 l An introduction to the ‘searchlight’ of attention;

l An introduction to the philosophy of mindfulness;

l Two mindful breathing exercises.

3 l A grounding mindfulness exercise;

l A discussion of ‘wobbly feelings’.

4 l The ‘count and add two’ mindful breathing exercise;

l A discussion of how to avoid reacting badly to situations.

5 l A discussion of worries;

l A discussion of how worries can be supported by the previously learnt

mindfulness exercises.

6 l A recap of learning;

l Practise all learnt mindfulness exercises.

of the four time-points. As the NEPSY-II Inhibition subtest includes several tasks, the

researchers selected the Naming and Inhibition Total Errors tasks due to them being

high in face validity relative to assessing

pupils sustaining and suppressing attention

skills, respectively (see Table 4). Although

the researchers considered using the TEACh (Manly et al., 2001) as in Napoli et al.’s

(2005) study, the NEPSY-II Inhibition subtest

appeared more sensitive to pupils’

suppressing and sustaining attention skills

and thus had superior face validity when

compared to the TEA-Ch.

The total number of errors made within

each of the two tasks are recorded and used

to generate one of seven percentile rank

categories relative to pupils’ age group: <2;

2–5; 6–10; 11–25; 26–50; 51–75; and >75.

The percentile rank categories were coded

so that pupils who made fewer errors

received higher scores. Hence, the lowest

percentile rank category (<2) received a

score of 1 and the highest percentile rank

category (>75) received a score of 7. Pre preintervention vs. pre-intervention withincondition comparisons were carried out in

the waitlist control group in order to rule out

the presence of practice effects and ensure

the validity of the measures.

In summary, three variables were created:

Attention Checklist; Naming Total Errors;

and, Inhibition Total Errors.

Findings

Teacher-reported Attention Checklist measure:

within-condition comparisons

Within the experimental group, Attention

Checklist scores increased significantly from

pre-intervention (median=42.5) to post-intervention (median=45). Whilst this intervention

effect was not maintained at the eight-week

follow-up (median=41.5), it was re-maintained

at the 14-week follow-up (median=46) with a

large estimated effect size1. Within the waitlist

control group, Attention Checklist scores did

not increase significantly from pre pre-intervention (median=38.5) to pre-intervention

(median=36.5), yet decreased significantly

from pre-intervention to post-intervention

(median=33.5). Whilst this inverse intervention effect was an unexpected finding, it was

not maintained at the six-week follow-up

(median=38). Findings achieving statistical

56 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

Table 4: Description of the Naming and the Inhibition Total Errors tasks.

Naming Total Errors task Inhibition Total Errors task

Shapes picture matrix Name each shape (i.e. ‘circle’/ Same procedure as the Naming

containing circles ‘square’) looking across each row Total Errors task, except pupils

and squares from left to right, starting with had to say ‘square’ when they saw

the top row and finishing with a circle and ‘circle’ when they saw

the bottom row. a square.

Arrows picture matrix State the direction of each arrow Same procedure as the Naming

containing arrows (i.e. ‘up’/ ‘down’) looking across Total Errors task, except pupils had

pointing up or down each row from left to right, to say ‘down’ then they saw an

starting with the top row and up arrow and ‘up’ when they saw

finishing with the bottom row. a down arrow.

1 An effect size is a standardised measure of the magnitude of an observed effect (Field, 2005, p.32). In the

present study, Rosenthal’s (1991, p.19) method was used to calculate estimated effect sizes using the formula

r=Z÷(√n). Hence, effect sizes are denoted by ‘r’. Given the present sample size was ≥28 but <85, Cohen (1992)

states that there was only sufficient statistical power to detect large estimated effect sizes (i.e. r≥.5).

significance on the Wilcoxon signed-rank test

(Wilcoxon, 1945) are indicated with an

asterisk (see Table 5).

Teacher-reported Attention Checklist measure:

between-condition comparisons

At Baseline, Attention Checklist scores within

the experimental group (median=42.5) did

not differ significantly from the waitlist

control group (median=38.5). At Time 1,

scores within the experimental group

(median=45) were significantly higher than

the waitlist control group (median=36.5), as

might have been predicted. However, unexpectedly the between-condition difference

remained at Time 2 (experimental group

median=41.5, waitlist control group

median=33.5) after the waitlist control group

had received Paws .b, and also remained at

Time 3 (experimental group median=46,

waitlist control group median=38). Findings

achieving statistical significance on the

Mann-Whitney test (Mann & Whitney, 1947)

are indicated with an asterisk (see Table 6).

Researcher-administered NEPSY-II measures:

within-condition comparisons

Within the experimental group, Naming

Total Errors scores increased significantly

from pre-intervention (median=4) to postintervention (median=6), meaning they

made fewer errors at post-intervention.

Furthermore, this intervention effect was

maintained at the eight-week follow-up

(median=6) and 14-week follow-up

(median=7) with large estimated effect sizes.

Within the waitlist control group, Naming

Total Errors scores did not increase

significantly from pre pre-intervention

(median=3.5) to pre-intervention

(median=4.5), yet increased significantly

from pre-intervention to post-intervention

(median=7). Nonetheless, this intervention

effect was not maintained at the six-week

follow-up (median=5.5).

Within the experimental group, Inhibition Total Errors scores increased significantly from pre-intervention (median=2.5)

to post-intervention (median=5). This interEducational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 57

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

Table 5: Teacher-reported measure inferential statistics: within-condition comparisons.

Variable Condition Pre pre- vs. pre- Pre- vs. post- Maintained at Maintained at

intervention intervention at follow-up 1 follow-up 2

Attention Experimental N/A p=.012* n.s. p=.001**

Checklist group r=–.53

Waitlist n.s. p=.039* n.s. N/A

control group (inverse)

Note: n.s.=not significant; *=p<.05; **=p<.01; N/A=impossible comparison due to the nature of the intervention cross-lag.

Table 6: Teacher-reported measure inferential statistics:

between-condition comparisons.

Variable Predicted outcome

No significant Significant difference at No significant No significant

difference at Time 1: Experimental difference at difference at

Baseline group > waitlist Time 2 Time 3

control group

Attention

n.s. p=.033* p=.039* p=.026* Checklist

Note: n.s.=not significant; *=p<.05; predicted outcomes have no shading; unpredicted outcomes are shaded grey.

vention effect was maintained at the eightweek follow-up (median=6) and 14-week

follow-up (median=5) with a large estimated

effect size at 14-week follow-up. Within the

waitlist control group, Inhibition Total

Errors scores did not increase significantly

from pre pre-intervention (median=4)

to pre-intervention (median=2.5), yet

increased significantly from pre-intervention

to post-intervention (median=6.5). Nonetheless, this intervention effect was not maintained at the six-week follow-up (median=6).

Findings achieving statistical significance on

the Wilcoxon signed-rank test are indicated

with an asterisk (see Table 7)

Researcher-administered NEPSY-II measures:

between-condition comparisons

At Baseline, Naming Total Errors scores

within the experimental group (median=4)

did not differ significantly from scores within

the waitlist control group (median=3.5). At

Time 1, scores within the experimental

group (median=6) were significantly higher

than scores within the waitlist control group

(median=4.5) and the between-condition

difference disappeared at Time 2 (experimental group median=6, waitlist control

group median=7), as might have been

predicted. However, the between-condition

difference unexpectedly reappeared at Time

3 (experimental group median=7, waitlist

control group median=5.5) (see Table 8)

At Baseline, Inhibition Total Errors

scores within the experimental group

(median=2.5) did not differ significantly

from scores within the waitlist control group

(median=4). However, unexpectedly the lack

of a between-condition difference remained

at Time 1 (experimental group median=5,

waitlist control group median=2.5), Time 2

(experimental group median=6, waitlist

control group median=6.5), and Time 3

(experimental group median=5, waitlist

control group median=6). Findings

achieving statistical significance on the

Mann-Whitney test are indicated with an

asterisk (see Table 8).

Discussion

There are several findings which provide

tentative evidence as to the positive impact

of Paws .b on mainstream primary-aged

pupils’ attentional functioning. Measures

taken from the Attention Checklist and the

Naming and Inhibition Total Errors tasks

indicated that Paws .b had a significantly

positive immediate and sustained impact

upon the attentional functioning of pupils in

the experimental group. According to the

Attention Checklist, the experimental

teacher observed pupils exhibiting more

58 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

Table 7: Researcher-administered measures inferential statistics:

within-condition comparisons

Variable Condition Pre pre- vs. pre- Pre- vs. post- Maintained at Maintained at

intervention intervention at follow-up 1 follow-up 2

Naming Experimental N/A p=.003** p=.002** p<.001**

Total group r=–.56 r=–.56

Errors Waitlist n.s. p=.022** n.s. N/A

control group

Inhibition Experimental N/A p=.015* p=.003** p=.001**

Total group r=–.52

Errors Waitlist n.s. p=.01* n.s. N/A

control group

Note: n.s.=not significant; *=p<.05; **=p<.01; N/A=impossible comparison due to the nature of the intervention cross-lag.

desirable suppressing and sustaining attention behaviours at post-intervention and

14-week follow-up when compared to preintervention. Likewise, according to Naming

and Inhibition Total Errors tasks, the experimental pupils were more accurate in their

use of sustaining and suppressing attention

skills at post-intervention and follow-up

when compared to pre-intervention. The

Naming and Inhibition Total Errors tasks

also indicated that Paws .b had a significantly

positive immediate impact upon the attentional functioning of pupils in the waitlist

control group and suggested that they were

more accurate in their use of sustaining and

suppressing attention skills at post-intervention when compared to pre-intervention.

Within the findings were two clear

patterns which are worthy of further discussion, both of which were unexpected

outcomes:

1. Intervention effects were stronger in the

experimental group than the waitlist

control group;

2. Follow-up effects within the experimental

group were stronger than immediate

intervention effects.

With regards to intervention effects being

stronger in the experimental group than the

waitlist control group, there are three

possible explanations. Firstly, the original

waitlist control teacher went on maternity

leave immediately after Time 1, meaning

that waitlist control pupils received Paws .b at

the same time as having to adjust to, and

build new relationships with, the second

waitlist control teacher. This disruption is

likely to have reduced the impact of Paws .b

within the waitlist control group.

Secondly, experimental and waitlist

control pupils were not matched according

to gender or core personality traits (e.g.

agreeableness/conscientiousness/openness

to experience; Norman, 1963). It is possible

that the gender differences between the

groups may have confounded the impact of

Paws .b, although research has yet to establish whether gender affects the impact of

mindfulness programmes, and given the

small sample sizes within each group, it is

questionable what impact gender differences may have had on the findings. Furthermore, had personality measures been taken,

these may have revealed differences between

the groups which may have affected the

impact of Paws .b.

Thirdly, pupils’ dispositional mindfulness

(the extent to which pupils naturally pay

attention on purpose, in the present

moment and non-judgementally) was not

assessed. Hence, the researchers were

unable to carry out mediation analyses and

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 59

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

Table 8: Researcher-administered measures inferential statistics:

between-condition comparisons.

Variable Predicted outcome

No significant Significant difference at No significant No significant

difference at Time 1: Experimental difference at difference at

Baseline group > waitlist Time 2 Time 3

control group

Naming

Total n.s. p=.009** n.s. p=.008**

Errors

Inhibition

Total n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.

Errors

Note: n.s.=not significant; *=p<.05; predicted outcomes have no shading; unpredicted outcomes are shaded grey.

ascertain whether pupils’ dispositional mindfulness mediated the relationship between

Paws .b and their suppressing and sustaining

attention skills. Therefore, intervention

effects within the experimental group may

have been stronger due to them having

greater dispositional mindfulness at preintervention.

With regards to follow-up effects within

the experimental group being stronger than

immediate intervention effects, there is one

possible explanation. With reference to

Haring and Eaton’s (1978) instructional

hierarchy of skill development, experimental pupils were able to acquire, become

fluent in their use of, and maintain their use

of, mindfulness skills throughout Paws .b.

Between post-intervention and follow-up,

experimental pupils would have had additional opportunities to generalise mindfulness skills in novel situations and adapt them

to suit their everyday lives. This may explain

why large estimated effect sizes were

observed at follow-up and not post-intervention.

Relative to previous research, the present

study replicated the findings of Napoli et al.

(2005). Both studies used a RCT design to

evaluate the impact of a manualised mindfulness programme on mainstream primaryaged pupils’ attentional functioning. Using

between-condition comparisons, both

studies found that experimental pupils had

significantly higher post-intervention attention scores than controls on teacherreported and researcher-administered

measures of attention. The present study

also made a number of methodological

improvements relative to Napoli et al.

(2005). Specifically, the present study: used a

teacher-reported attention measure tailored

towards observable attention behaviours (i.e.

the Attention Checklist) as opposed to

ADHD symptoms (i.e. the ACTeRS);

included baseline between-condition

comparisons to ensure that the experimental

and control groups did not differ significantly at the outset; and, included follow-up

assessment.

However, it should be noted that, despite

attempts to introduce methodological rigour,

the present study illustrates some of the

issues facing EPs as real-world researchers in

trying to implement quasi-experimental

designs. From a positive perspective, the use

of a RCT with a quasi-experimental intervention cross-lag enabled the researchers to

establish a link between the introduction of

Paws .b and associated changes in pupils’

attentional functioning, as well as reduce

possible Hawthorne effects by ensuring that

that Paws .b was delivered to all pupils by the

same mindfulness teacher. However, the use

of a RCT in a real-world setting was not only

difficult to negotiate with school stakeholders, but was difficult to implement in

practice. As there was a lack of a double-blind

element to the design, class teachers and the

first researcher knew when each group of

pupils were receiving Paws .b, which may, in

turn, have increased possible Hawthorne

effects by affecting their ratings/ assessments

of pupils attentional functioning. Furthermore, the present study failed to achieve

Greenberg and Harris’ (2012) criterion for a

long-term follow-up of six months.

Upon critiquing the sample used within

the present study, the researchers were

restricted to inferring only large estimated

effect sizes because of the small number of

pupils and the experimental and waitlist

control groups were unmatched according

to gender and core personality traits. The

generalisability of the findings were low due

to the sample consisting entirely of pupils

who had EAL and due to the study taking

place in a single context. Furthermore, there

was unavoidable attrition of the original waitlist control group teacher. Although steps

were taken to protect the validity of the

Attention Checklist dataset within the waitlist control group, the validity of comparing

the Attention Checklist data generated by

the original waitlist control teacher against

the data generated by the second waitlist

control teacher is questionable and may

explain why an inverse intervention effect

was observed.

60 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

It is the researchers’ view that the single

largest difficulty within the present study was

controlling for extraneous variables, relative

to its quasi-experimental design. For

example, it is very likely that the factor which

most affected the impact of Paws .b on the

waitlist control group (and subsequently the

RCT design as whole) was their change of

class teacher. Campbell and Stanley (1963)

state that quasi-experimental designs allow

researchers to control the ‘who and to whom of

measurement’, but lack control over the ‘when

and to whom of exposure’, where randomly

assigning pupils to experimental and control

groups is an essential component of an RCT

design. However, in a real-world setting such

as a school, randomisation is often unacceptable and, therefore, it is difficult to exclude

the possibility of confounding variables

(Robitaille et al., 2012).

The findings may be useful to EPs in light

of the growing interest in mindfulness-based

interventions (Davis, 2012). The study

provides tentative evidence that, for schools,

Paws .b offers a means of improving the

attentional functioning of their pupils. Paws

.b also has the potential to provide additional support for individual and groups of

pupils with attentional difficulties such as

ADHD. At the whole-class level, Paws .b may

offer a means of enriching environmental

input within the classroom in a way that

enables pupils to realise their underlying

attentional potential (see Ruff & Rothbart,

1986; Schweizer et al., 2005), which may

promote their cognitive development and

academic progress (see Breslau et al., 2010;

Steele et al., 2012). Regarding educational

psychology practice, the researchers believe

that EPs are well placed to support schools’

implementation of preventative interventions such as Paws .b rather than necessarily

delivering mindfulness interventions directly

(see Fallon, Woods & Rooney, 2010).

There are numerous possible directions

for future research suggested by this paper.

It might be possible to replicate the present

study, and extend its findings by making

appropriate methodological adjustments

(for instance, by including a double-blind

element and a six-month follow-up within

the design; recruiting a larger sample of

pupils who are able to be matched according

to gender and core personality traits; or

recruiting a larger sample of class teachers to

help reduce the potential impact of attrition), or by replicating it with a sample of

non-EAL pupils in a different context. Other

directions include assessing whether pupils’

dispositional mindfulness mediates the

impact of Paws .b, or assessing the impact of

Paws .b on the attentional functioning of a

clinical group of pupils, such as those with

ADHD. It might also be interesting to

compare the impact of Paws .b against more

and less intensive mindfulness programmes,

as well as other forms of attention state

training, regarding pupils’ attentional functioning, or to observe the impact of Paws .b

at the neural level (using Functional

Magnetic Resonance Imaging, for example).

Address for correspondence

Cathy Atkinson

Doctorate in Educational and Child

Psychology,

Floor 5, Ellen Wilkinson Building,

The University of Manchester,

Manchester M13 9PL.

Email: Cathy.Atkinson@manchester.ac.uk

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 61

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

Anderson, V., Northam, E., Hendy, J. & Wrennall, J.

(2002). Developmental neuropsychology: A clinical

approach. Hove: Psychology Press.

Barnett, D., Hawkins, R. & Lentz Jr., F.E. (2011).

Intervention adherence for researcher and practice: Necessity or triage outcome? Journal of

Educational and Psychological Consultation, 21(3),

175–190.

Bishop, S.R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L.,

Anderson, N.D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z.V., Abbey,

S., Speca, M., Velting, D. & Devins, G. (2004).

Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition.

Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11,

230–241.

Breslau, N., Breslau, J., Peterson, E., Miller, E., Lucia,

V.C., Bohnert, K. & Nigg, J. (2010). Change in

teachers’ ratings of attention problems and

subsequent change in academic achievement:

A prospective analysis. Psychological Medicine, 40,

159–166.

Campbell, D.T. & Stanley, J. (1963). Experimental and

quasi-experimental designs for research on teaching.

Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological

Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159.

Cooley, E.L. & Morris, R.D. (1990). Attention in

children: A neuropsychologically-based model

for assessment. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6,

239–274.

Das, J.P. (2002). Attention: Concept, tests and

teacher ratings. Psychology and Developing Societies,

14(2), 241–260.

Davis, T.S. (2012). Mindfulness-based approaches

and their potential for educational psychology

practice. Educational Psychology in Practice, 28(1),

31–46.

Department for Education (DfE) (2013). The school

curriculum. Retrieved 22 November 2013, from:

http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/

teachingandlearning/curriculum.

Durlak, J.A. & DuPre, E.P. (2008). Implementation

matters: A review of research on the influence of

implementation on program outcomes and the

factors affecting implementation. American

Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 327–350.

Elkins, I.J., Malone, S., Keyes, M., Iacono, W.G. &

McGue, M. (2011). The impact of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder on preadolescent

adjustment may be greater for girls than boys.

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,

40(4), 532–545.

Fallon, K., Woods, K. & Rooney, S. (2010). A discussion of the developing role of educational

psychologists within Children’s Services. Educational Psychology in Practice, 26(1), 1–23.

Felver, J.C., Tipsord, J.M., Morris, M.J., Racer, K.H. &

Dishion, T.J. (2014). The effects of mindfulnessbased intervention on children’s attention regulation. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1–10.

Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS

(2nd ed.). London: Sage.

Ford, T., Goodman, R. & Meltzer, H. (2003). The

British child and adolescent mental health survey

1999: The prevalence of DSM-IV disorders.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1203–1211.

Goodman, T.A. (2005). Working with children:

Beginner’s mind. In C.K. Germer, R.D. Siegel &

P.R. Fulton (Eds.), Mindfulness and psychotherapy.

New York: The Guilford Press.

Greenberg, M.T. & Harris, A.R. (2012). Nurturing

mindfulness and children and youth: Current

state of research. Child Development Perspectives,

6(2), 161–166.

Haring, N.G. & Eaton, M.D. (1978). Systematic

instructional procedures: An instructional hierarchy. In N.G. Haring, T.C. Lovitt, M.D. Eaton &

C.L. Hansen (Eds.), The Fourth R: Research in the

classroom. Ohio: Charles E. Merrill.

Huppert, F.A. & Johnson, D.M. (2010). A controlled

trial of mindfulness training in schools: The

importance of practice for an impact on wellbeing. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(4),

264–274.

Jones, D. (2011). Mindfulness in schools. The Psychologist, 24(10), 736–739.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the

wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and

illness. New York: Delacorte.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2000). Indra’s net at work: The mainstreaming of Dharma practice in society. York Beach,

ME: Samuel Weiser.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present and future. Clinical

Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 44–56.

Klassen, A.F., Miller, A. & Fine, S. (2004). Healthrelated quality of life in children and adolescents

who have diagnoses of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 114(5), 541–547.

Korkman, M., Kirk, U. & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY–II.

Pearson Assessment.

Kostanski, M. & Hassed, C. (2008). Mindfulness as a

concept and a process. Australian Psychologist,

43(1), 15–21.

Kuyken, W., Weare, K., Ukoumunne, O.C., Vicary, R.,

Motton, N., Burnett, R., Cullen, C., Hennelly, S.

& Huppert, F. (2013). Effectiveness of the Mindfulness in Schools Programme: Non-randomised

controlled feasibility study. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 203(2), 126–131.

62 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

References

Lara, C., Fayyad, J., de Graaf, R., Kessler, R.C.,

Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Angermeyer, M., Demytteneare, K., de Girolamo, G., Haro, J.M., Jin, R.,

Karam, E.G., Lepine, J.P., Mora, M.E., Ormel, J.,

Posada-Villa, J. & Sampson, N. (2009). Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from the World Health

Organisation world mental health survey initiative. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 46–54.

Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B. & Loring, D.W. (2004).

Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Lubelska, A. (2012). Project report: Peaceful schools.

International Journal of Children’s Spirituality,

17(2), 187–191.

Luria, A.R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic

Books.

Lutz, A., Slagter, H.A., Dunne, J. & Davidson, R.J.

(2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in

meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4),

163–169.

MacKay, T. (2005). The relationship of educational

psychology and clinical neuropsychology. Educational and Child Psychology, 22(2), 7–17.

Manly, T., Nimmo-Smith, I., Watson, P., Anderson, V.,

Turner, A. & Robertson, I.H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The

Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEACh): Normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42,

1065–1081.

Mann, H.B. & Whitney, D.R. (1947). On a test of

whether one or two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. Annals of Mathematical

Statistics, 18, 50–60.

Melbourne Academic Mindfulness Interest Group

(MAMIG) (2006). Mindfulness-based psychotherapies: A review of conceptual foundations,

empirical evidence and practical considerations.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,

40, 285–294.

Mindfulness in Schools Project (2013a). Paws .b.

Retrieved 22 November 2013, from:

http://mindfulnessinschools.org/

courses/paws-b/.

Mindfulness in Schools Project (2013b). .b for Teens.

Retrieved 22 November 2013, from:

http://mindfulnessinschools.org/

courses/b-for-teens/.

Mirsky, A.F., Anthony, B.J., Duncan, C.C., Ahearn,

M.B. & Kellam, S.G. (1991). Analysis of the

elements of attention: A neuropsychological

approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2(2), 109–145.

Napoli, M., Krech, P.R. & Holley, L.C. (2005). Mindfulness training for elementary school students:

The attention academy. Journal of Applied School

Psychology, 21(1), 99–125.

Norman, W.T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy

of personality attributes: Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings.

Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66,

574–583.

Petersen, S.E. & Posner, M.I., (2012). The attention

system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual

Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.

Robitaille, Y., Fournier, M., Laforest, S., Gauvin, L.,

Filiatrault, J. & Corriveau, H. (2012). Effect of a

fall prevention programme on balance maintenance using a quasi-experimental design in realworld setting. Journal of Ageing and Health, 24,

827–845.

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social

research (revised). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ruff, H. & Rothbart, M. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Schweizer, K., Moosbrugger, H. & Goldhammer, F.

(2005). The structure of the relationship

between attention and intelligence. Intelligence,

33, 589–611.

Semple, R.J., Lee, J., Rosa, D. & Miller, L.F. (2010).

A randomised trial of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for children: Promoting mindful

attention to enhance social-emotional resiliency

in children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19,

218–229.

Steele, A., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Cornish, K. & Scerif,

G. (2012). The multiple subfunctions of attention: differential developmental gateways to

literacy and numeracy. Child Development, 83(6),

2028–2041.

Strauss, M.E., Thompson, P., Adams, N.L., Redline, S.

& Burant, C. (2000). Evaluation of a model of

attention with confirmatory factor analysis.

Neuropsychology, 14(2), 201–208.

Stuss, D.T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M.P. & Picton,

T.W. (1995). A multidisciplinary approach to

anterior attentional functions. Annals of the

New York Academy of Science, 769, 191–211.

Tang, Y.Y. & Posner, M.I. (2009). Attention training

and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive

Sciences, 13(5), 222–227.

Thomas, G.L. (2013). Assignment 1: Special Educational

Needs Co-ordinators’ views regarding the attentional

components most commonly used by mainstream

primary school aged children and the attentional

components that the primary school curriculum makes

most demands on. University of Manchester:

D.Ed.Ch.Psychol. Research Paper 1.

Ullmann, R.K., Sleator, E.K. & Sprague, R.L. (1997).

ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating Scale

(ACTeRS). Champaign, IL: MetriTech, Inc.

Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1 63

Measuring the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based intervention for children’s attentional functioning

van der Oord, S., Bögels, S. M. & Peijnenburg, D.

(2012). The effectiveness of mindfulness training

for children with ADHD and mindful parenting

for their parents. Journal of Child and Family

Studies, 21, 139–147.

Ward, J. (2006). The student’s guide to cognitive neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press.

Wilcoxon, F. (1945). Individual comparisons by

ranking methods. Biometrics, 1, 80–83.

Williams, M. & Penman, D. (2011). Mindfulness:

A practical guide to finding peace in a frantic world.

London: Piatkus.

64 Educational & Child Psychology Vol. 33 No. 1

George Thomas & Cathy Atkinson

Copyright of Educational & Child Psychology is the property of British Psychological Society

and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without

the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or

email articles for individual use.

Pearls, Pith, and Provocation

Qualitative Health Research

19(10) 1495–1503

© The Author(s) 2009

Reprints and permission: http://www.

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

DOI: 10.1177/1049732309348500

http://qhr.sagepub.com

Understanding the Ecological Validity

of Neuropsychological Testing Using

an Ethnographic Approach

Deborah Gioia1

Abstract

Neurocognitive impairment is a defining and disabling feature of schizophrenia and other physical disorders. Most of

our understanding about neurocognitive deficits comes from laboratory-based testing in research protocols. There

has been little research using direct behavioral community observation over a prolonged period to understand the

association of daily functioning with cognitive performance. The purpose of this study was to develop an observational

method that could be replicated by researchers interested in viewing cognitive deficits in vivo, and then comparing

this data to laboratory measures to affirm the ecological validity of those measures. The eight-step method explained

here was developed from the targeted ethnographic study of 10 persons with schizophrenia. Obtaining real world

context with this method will help to increase the generalizability of effective cognitive treatments, create improved

interventions for this population, and bring into greater relief the coping and compensatory strategies already used by

individuals to complete daily tasks.

Keywords

ethnography; mental health and illness; neuropsychology

Ecological validity has become an increasingly important

notion in science, particularly when we want to understand how individuals who have one outcome from

neuropsychological (NP) laboratory testing (e.g., low

functioning) seem to have more varied outcomes (e.g.,

high functioning in some tasks, low in others) when they

are in their everyday settings. Ecological validity has been

defined as “the functional and predictive relationship

between the patient’s performance on a set of neuropsychological tests and the patient’s behavior in a variety of

real-world settings” (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004;

Sbordone, 1996, p. 16). There have been difficulties in

relating NP findings and behavioral laboratory measures

to real-world outcomes. An ethnographic approach to

ecological validity will provide new information about

how cognitive impairments impact the lives of persons

affected with NP-involved disorders (e.g., schizophrenia,

multiple sclerosis) in a more direct way than self-report,

clinician ratings, or simulation of a real-world event like

planning to shop for items to make a cake, rather than

actually making the cake. An eight-step ethnographic

method for assessing ecological validity is the focus of

this article.

There are legitimate and important reasons for pursuing a better understanding of ecological validity of

neuropsychological tests. Previous literature has argued

that what is learned in the laboratory about cognitive

functioning might be too artificial or narrow to make

meaningful predictions about how an individual will

respond in a given situation when they have cognitive

decline or deficit (Groth-Marnat, 2000; Koren, Seidman,

Goldsmith, & Harvey, 2006; Sbordone, 1996). Despite

this, NP tests originally developed to provide information

about diagnosis are being used to answer questions about

everyday functioning, with very little empirical evidence

to support this practice (Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe,

2003). When a lab test is administered, all real-world

context is stripped from the task so as to complete the test

with as few distractions as possible. In daily life, distractions and complications abound with even the simplest

task, so this artificial performance does not tell us much.

We know that individuals with NP deficits demonstrate

much creativity and resiliency in the face of these realworld distractions, but we have not had a good way to

1

University of Maryland, Baltimore, Maryland, USA

Corresponding Author:

Deborah Gioia, University of Maryland, School of Social Work, 525 W.

Redwood St., Baltimore, MD 21201, USA

E-mail: dgioia@ssw.umaryland.edu

1496 Qualitative Health Research 19(10)

capture these data and compare them to NP test results in

a distraction-free environment.

Groth-Marnat (2000) reviewed the qualitative/quantitative continuum of assessment as it relates to the

understanding of the individual client. The author noted

the divide between these assessment approaches with

regard to the external validity of NP tests. He stated,

“Test validity traditionally has not focused on a person’s

everyday abilities/characteristics but rather on such areas

as correlation with previously developed measures, factor

analyses, and level of diagnostic agreement” (GrothMarnat, 2000, p. 357). One hopes that a fuller integration

of testing and behavioral observation, coupled with the

incorporation of emerging technology and theory, and

research discoveries would lead us to a richer understanding of the functioning of the individual in context, and

provide a means to get needed interventions and services

to individuals in the community in an expedited manner

(Bellack et al, 2007; Brekke, Ell, & Palinkas, 2007;

National Institute of Mental Health [NIMH], 2008).

Rather than attribute real-world performance or prediction of future performance to any single neurocognitive

test or proxy measure, which is frequently implied but is

a weak measure of prediction (Sbordone, 1996), this proposed method captures a dimension that has not been

integrated in the ecological validity or prediction literature.

Prior to articulating the steps of the method, a brief

review of ecological validity studies and proxy measures

of NP functioning are described.

Neuropsychological Assessment

and Everyday Life

Chaytor and Schmitter-Edgecombe (2003) reviewed the

empirical studies in which ecological validity of neuropsychological (NP) tests was assessed against individuals’

use of everyday skills and found that the magnitude of

relationships between individual tests and measures of

outcome was weak. The studies reviewed mainly had

populations diagnosed with closed head injuries and

chronic medical conditions (e.g., multiple sclerosis), and

only two studies looked at individuals with schizophrenia.

However, none of the outcome measures used in any of

the studies was a direct behavioral observation of the

person in the community; rather, outcomes relied mainly

on clinician ratings, self- and informant reports, and

questionnaires. This review highlighted that our understanding to date of how persons with cognitive deficits

make decisions and function in their homes, social environments, and service entities is largely unknown.

Illness- and injury-specific studies abound in the NP

assessment literature. In Alzheimer’s research, the ability

to predict self-neglect in community functioning from NP

tests has emerged as promising, but only end-state negative outcomes (e.g., hospitalization or report to adult

protective services) were correlated with tests (Tierney,

Snow, Charles, Moineddin, & Kiss, 2007). A study comparing traumatic brain-injured (TBI) and normal adult

subjects in their self-reported slips of attention (e.g.,

burning the toast, throwing out the vegetables with the

peels) demonstrated in part that there was not a lot of difference between the two groups on a particular neurocognitive

measure of attention and in their self-reported slips of attention, as was hypothesized (Robertson, Manly, Andrade,

Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997). These studies and others illustrate the elusiveness of reliable or standardized predictions

in heterogeneous populations. Without a working knowledge of the actual demands that the environment places on

individuals with or without cognitive impairment, without

an emphasis on and understanding of context, predictions

about community functioning are tenuous, at best. This

overall poor prediction of community abilities based on

any specific NP tests has led researchers to consider other

assessment methodologies, namely proxy measures.

Proxy Approaches to

Neuropsychological Assessment

Thus far, ecological validity in the community has been

assessed with tasks that are completed and rated in office

settings but are crafted as simulations of real-world activities and decisions. However, many of these tasks or

activities assessing an individual’s functioning are limited approximations of real-world tasks. Three examples

of proxy measurement from schizophrenia research are: (a)

the University of California, San Diego Performance-based

Skills Assessment (UPSA; Patterson, Goldman, McKibbin,

Hughs, & Jeste, 2001). This assessment directly examines

participants’ abilities to perform “instrumental” activities of

daily living (e.g., preparing a shopping list, planning a bus

trip), but does so in an office environment and yields a summary score of 0 to 100 to assess overall NP performance.

A second NP proxy assessment outside the clinic setting involves having an individual perform tasks in a

natural setting (e.g., supermarket) using a standardized

rating scale, such as the Test of Grocery Shopping Skills

(TOGSS; Brown, Rempfer, Hamera, & Bothwell, 2006;

Greenwood, Landau, & Wykes, 2005; Rempfer, Hamera,

Brown, & Cromwell, 2003). The grocery trip is organized by the researcher and the actions of the individual

in response to the test directions are rated and quantitatively scored. Following are three examples of proxy

measurement from schizophrenia research. The first is

the University of California, San Diego Performancebased Skills Assessment (UPSA; Patterson, Goldman,

McKibbin, Hughs, & Jeste, 2001).

Gioia 1497

A third approach to ecological validity uses an experience sampling method (ESM) to get closer to cognitive

impairments at the time they occur (Myin-Germeys,

Delespaul, & Van Os, 2005). Participants were given a

programmed digital wristwatch and self-assessment forms

to complete on their thoughts, current context, and mood

when the watch alarm activated. Current iterations of this

method involve using personal digital assistants (PDAs)

to prompt participants to answer particular questions

while going about their activities in the community

(Granholm, Loh, & Swendson, 2008). Although this

method captures a wider breadth of activities than supermarket shopping, and is linked to ongoing community

behavior, it does not provide direct observations of

behavior in natural contexts and precludes direct questioning by the researcher about activities as they occur.

Although proxy measures represent advances in shifting assessment and skill measurement to community

settings, they fall short in the application of direct observation of behavior in the places where individuals live

and work. The eight-step model to capture real-world

functioning presented here is not hypothetical, but one

that has been used in funded National Institute of Mental

Health (NIMH) research to explore ecological validity

(Gioia, 2006; Gioia & Brekke, 2009). The basic features

and steps of the model are presented as explicitly as possible so as to highlight key contributions to make it

accessible and easily replicated in future studies.

Study Information

Summarized information about the study sample and

design from which this ecological validity method was

developed is provided here, and is more fully reported

elsewhere (Gioia, 2006; Gioia & Brekke, 2009). The

present study was approved by the Institutional Review

Board for the University of Southern California and

funded by NIMH. The participants (n = 10) were all

adults, including 5 men and 5 women with schizophrenia

spectrum disorders, and were originally enrolled in a longitudinal study, “Predicting Psychosocial Rehabilitation

Service Outcomes” (n = 140; Brekke, Raine, Ansel,

Lencz, & Bird, 1997), which was the parent study for this

sample. Participants had completed all data collection

with the parent-study protocol prior to being approached

for this study. They all signed new consent forms that

indicated their voluntary participation and the procedures

for protecting their anonymity. Consent procedures for

vulnerable populations used in the parent-study protocol

were followed to ensure comprehension of the form,

including reading the form to the participant when necessary because of limited literacy or lack of reading glasses.

Participants understood that they could withdraw from

the study at any time with no impact to or loss of their

current mental health services. Individuals were paid for

their participation ($20/day), and their decisions about

how to use their study payment became part of the study

data.

The parent-study participants were divided into three

groups based on their NP composite scores (low, midrange, and high functioning). The completed NP tasks

that led to distinguishing these groups included measures

of attention, memory, problem solving, processing speed,

language, and recall, which are typically used in NP batteries for persons with schizophrenia. The mid-range

group (30%) was omitted from the sampling frame so as

to maximize the two extremes of functioning (high vs.

low) for the observation. I was blind to the neurocognitive status of the 10 study participants during the

observations. Participant housing ranged from fully independent apartments, to living with family, to congregate

living, and the length of time in these settings varied.

Because the parent study measured service characteristics, all 10 participants had some current linkage to

community mental health programs, all were taking medication—though some were not fully managing their own

medication schedule, and none were on long-lasting

injectable medication. Complete sampling procedures,

study measures, and sample characteristics from the

parent study are reported elsewhere (Gioia & Brekke,

2009). Although all participants had the option of discontinuing the study at any time, none chose to do so.

The following general points about the 10 individuals

in the study provide some demographic context: (a) All

individuals were on oral antipsychotics and often other

medication, managed by themselves or family members;

(b) none reported abusing substances at the time of the

study, and no toxicology screens were used to corroborate the self-report; (c) all were connected to some level

of community mental health services; (d) all were housed

from independent living to group facilities; (e) all had some

type of regular income (including disability payments),

although all were living in tight financial situations and

had little discretionary income; (f) all had some supportive relationships in their lives—but not necessarily family;

and (g) none were currently in a cognitive remediation

program to remedy their deficits.

The study design was a mixed-method triangulation

convergence design as articulated by Creswell and Plano

Clark (2007). Qualitative and quantitative data were

viewed as having equal importance in the model. The

analysis was an integration of individual participant NP

scores, standardized measures, and ethnographic data;

individual data were analyzed, compared, and contrasted.

Researchers typically use this design when they want to

compare results or to validate, confirm, or corroborate

1498 Qualitative Health Research 19(10)

quantitative results with qualitative findings (Creswell &

Plano Clark, 2007). Although not typically a qualitative

procedure, the decision for this study was to blind the

field interviewer to the high/low NP status of the individuals, thus removing any possible source of researcher

bias that might occur if the cognitive status was known.

Use of the blind was a novel design feature that should be

considered for fit in relation to one’s study. It is not

included in the model steps presented here, but use of the

blind was felt to enhance the rigor of the findings.

The main question for this study was whether there

was a difference in those individuals who had high

and low scores on their laboratory tests and their functioning in the community. Daily tasks and weekly or

monthly tasks were observed within a 3-day period. These

included tasks and activities such as grocery shopping,

eating out (as in a restaurant), doing laundry, scheduling

appointments, and using public transportation. Interest

was not only in observing the tasks as they unfolded

during the follow-along observation in the community;

there was also interest in individuals’ descriptions (upon

being asked) of what they felt about their capabilities/

strategies in completing daily tasks, and their personal

recognition of deficits. These conversations usually took

place at the end of the task (e.g., arriving home from the

market), and were considered part of the individual data.

Steps in Ecological Validity Assessment

Neuropsychological testing can either be completed as a

full battery prior to enrolling in an observation study, or

one can use a brief community-based neuropsychological

assessment such as The Repeatable Battery of the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS; Randolph,

1998), which can be administered at the start of the study

in a community setting, and which takes approximately

30 minutes to complete. Test results from either type of

battery can be calculated as a single summary score to

predetermine cutoff ranges for high and low NP functioning levels in the analysis of the qualitative data.

The following eight steps were found to be useful in

the present study (Gioia, 2006; Gioia & Brekke, 2009),

which was designed for this small sample but has applicability for subsamples of much larger studies.

Many of the steps of the model were identified in the

design phase, whereas others were adopted during data

collection. Although the method is similar in many ways

to participant observation, the difference is that the intent

of the observations is always to follow the cognitive strategies the individual employs in relation to how they

perceive their illness deficits. This requires that the field

researcher has background and training in recognizing

basic cognitive deficits and inquiring when they are displayed. A descriptive guide to the steps in the model is

presented.

Step 1: Getting to Know the Participant

This critical step makes use of the initial time spent getting to know the individual at their service site, by getting

to know the individual’s routines through the gathering of

basic demographic, symptom, and psychosocial data (the

Table 1. Eight Steps in the Ecological Validity Process

Step

1. Getting to know the participant

2. Utilizing prolonged engagement with participants

3. Encouraging participant-led activities with opportunity

for reflection

4. Taking field notes

5. Having contact with significant others and community

members

6. Coding and categorizing observed behaviors

and strategies

7. Employing intercoder agreement as a means to

strengthen validity of the data

8. Using the cocreated results

Researcher Task

Develop rapport, collect quantitative data, schedule community

observation

Spend 3 consecutive days viewing typical and atypical tasks

Build in time for participant reflection on use of metacognitive

processes

Write notes in a timely manner at the end of each field day

Check for gaps in understanding with members of team and return

to field to check understanding with participants

Determine depth of interaction with other individuals, as they can

provide collateral information on functioning

Use IADL language to create categories that can be used as a

template for additional coders to rate

Use multiple coders for field notes and have them code notes

according to types of tasks

Generate intercoder reliability scores

Results should be checked with the participants for accuracy

Results should be shared with scientists working in neuropsychology to increase understanding of ecological validity

Gioia 1499

quantitative data; see Gioia & Brekke, 2009). This step

occurs after consent to participate in the study has been

obtained. During this rapport-building phase, a plan for

the researcher to join the individual in their community is

cocreated; this is not a simple process and might be taxing

for the participant. Carlsson, Paterson, Scott-Findlay,

Ehnfors, and Ehrenberg (2007) discuss multiple strategies

for interviewing participants who have communication

and cognitive difficulties (e.g., recognizing fatigue). The

researcher needs to take ample notes as the potential participant describes what they will be doing over the next

few days. If there is a generic activity that the researcher

wants to observe with each person (e.g., money management), and the person has not mentioned this activity,

these areas will be raised to see when this activity might

next come up for them (e.g., when their monthly check

arrives). Observations should be scheduled in the least

obtrusive way possible. Evidence for rapport-building

would be demonstrated by the participant’s trust in the

initial process and by their adequate communication

skills. There is no formal measure for this kind of engagement. Exclusion would be based on high levels of

psychiatric symptoms that interfere with rapport (e.g.,

manic or highly anxious behavior, extreme paranoia). In

this initial visit it is also critical to have the researcher ask

the individual how he or she wants to introduce the

researcher to others as he or she accompanies them in the

community. The introduction might take one form for

close personal friends and family, and another for neighbors and mental health professionals. Rehearsing these

introductions might be helpful.

Step 2: Utilizing Prolonged Engagement

With Participants

To create credible findings for ecological validity of NP

assessments, one needs to spend ample time with each

individual to allow for opportunities to view a range of

community activities, some of them repeatedly. The

method employed here is somewhere between rapid ethnographic assessment and a full ethnography. The rapid

ethnographic assessment is a technique first developed

by Scrimshaw, Carballo, Ramos, and Blair (1991) in HIV

studies to yield contextual data in a short period of time

to obtain information from participants that will likely be

explored in a full qualitative study. A full ethnography

involves a thorough cultural immersion that involves

staying in people’s lives for an extended period of time,

usually months to years (Hammersley & Atkinson, 1995).

The method presented here calls for a period of initial

immersion (9 to 15 hours over 3 consecutive days,

approximately 3 to 5 hours per day; weekends and evenings are possible) viewing participant-led activities, and

most importantly, honing in on specific tasks that are part

of instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs; Lawton

& Brody, 1969). IADLs are activities such as using the

telephone, getting to places beyond walking distance (use

of transportation), grocery shopping, meal preparation,

household chores, laundry, taking medications, and managing money. The IADLs form the foundation of observation

for all populations with cognitive decline, and thus serve

as a baseline of common tasks for analysis. In addition,

each individual has her or his own interests (e.g., using

computers, crafts and model making, attending church),

which provide additional opportunities to view a wide

range of complex tasks beyond IADLs. Every opportunity should be made to attend these additional activities,

even though some might be out of the immediate community in which the participant lives. Activities that

cannot be viewed in the initial 3 consecutive days, or

those needing additional observation, will be scheduled

with the participant when they naturally occur.

Step 3: Encouraging Participant-Led Activities

With Opportunity for Reflection

Once the observation of the participant begins there are

many opportunities to use questions during the task or

after the completion of a complex task—such as ordering

a meal in a restaurant—to probe some of the specific task

components. For example, one individual in the study

ordered a meal in a fast food restaurant and chose a milkshake that he did not like, which created a dilemma for

him. Questions posed by the researcher at the moment of

the event offered the individual a chance to think aloud

with the researcher listening, and decide what to do next

(e.g., keep the shake or request a new one). This example

of an event offered a distinct opportunity to view problem

solving as it unfolded. The technique is similar to that

used in cognitive interviewing when individuals are commenting on the development of a measure (Willis, 2005).

By observing events in real time, there is an increased

opportunity to discover how the person creates a plan of

action around the various tasks, to witness their emotional

state while doing the task, as well as to examine their

decision making about the particular action. Metacognitive processes are often called “thoughts about thinking,”

and can enhance the observed experience in ways not

tapped by observation or tests alone (Lysaker et al,

2005). Obtaining reflection from the participant serves

as a clarification about what the interviewer observed

and also serves as a validity check of the method.

Questions about awareness of deficits in thought or

action can be asked. Enough information needs to be

solicited to determine whether any struggles observed are

because of a social emotional context (anxiety, fear) or

1500 Qualitative Health Research 19(10)

whether it was difficult for the individual to conceptualize the skills necessary to negotiate the task (e.g., talking

with a waiter about returning an incorrect order). The

model presented here captures both the strengths and

difficulties of the individual with neurocognitive deficits

in the community context.

Step 4: Taking Field Notes

The least obtrusive way to collect data about everyday

activities is through the use of field observation and

notes. Wolcott (1995), in The Art of Fieldwork, provided

suggestions to help the researcher “be there” in the field.

He does not recommend that the observer be aloof, but

rather that the observer constantly challenge him- or herself about the observed behavior, which is a great fit for

observing and understanding cognitive deficits. The strategies of field work are mainly nondirective, but there is a

flow between asking direct questions and asking no questions at all, which feeds into the art of this procedure.

There is no absolute way to conduct a field study (van

Manen, 1988).

As soon as possible after the observation is completed,

the observer should write up notes that describe the

events in abundant detail, which will refresh your

memory when you return to the notes. The write-up

should occur prior to the next scheduled observation,

otherwise events will blur and crucial details might be

lost. Observed events directly related to NP should be

listed and taken with the researcher back to the field, to

assist in understanding whether the observation was isolated or more frequently occurring. Consultation with a

neuropsychologist is useful in this stage, as well as in the

ongoing data analysis. Digital audio recorders used postinterview can help to get main thoughts down quickly,

and details can be added when doing the full write-up of

the observation.

Step 5: Having Contact With Significant Others

and Community Members

During the observation it is likely that the individual will

have social contact with others in her or his community,

which is an important area of functioning to observe.

Mental health professionals might be included in these

community contacts. The additional information from

these significant others could prove useful as collaborative information, especially in understanding their role in

helping the individual with daily tasks. These persons can

serve as primary resources for the individual completing

their daily routine tasks. They can provide an additional

source of data to triangulate; however, this would require

a separate set of permission/consent forms. It is important

to negotiate with the individual in the study how these

transactions should be made.

Step 6: Coding and Categorizing Observed

Behaviors and Strategies

One key use of ecological validity is as an assessment

tool, because the integrated data collected can be used to

make determinations or predictions about functional outcomes. One rubric for analysis is to use predominant

behavioral strategies as used in IADLs for describing

observed tasks. In this study, in developing the ecological validity method, four types of strategies were used:

(a) instrumental, (b) rote, (c) social facilitative, and

(d) social independent. Although there is overlap in these

strategies, the analysis called for making an overall judgment about the predominating behavioral style.

Rote activities enable basic community living (such as

doing chores), but are largely characterized by repetitious

or scripted behavior that is often, but not always, demonstrated and guided by others. Instrumental tasks are most

aligned with multiple activities of daily living—self-care,

shopping and other activities related to food, making and

keeping appointments, keeping clothes clean—and are

generally more complex, goal-oriented, and a bit more

self-directed. Facilitative social activities are social connections formed with people to facilitate daily functioning,

usually of the more complex instrumental tasks (e.g.,

asking someone to help with budgeting, with transportation, or advice about decisions). Independent social

activities are social connections and interactions that are

actively cultivated by the individual and that enrich work,

school, living, community, and family tasks, but the individual is not largely dependent on others for completion

of tasks or for direction (Gioia & Brekke, 2009). Other

rubrics or templates for analysis are likely to be developed with the addition of more individual observations.

Step 7: Employing Intercoder Agreement as a

Means to Strengthen Validity of the Data

Participant profiles are created from the descriptions of

tasks in the field notes as a means to reduce the data into

a document that can be read, coded, and rated by multiple

coders. These are basically summary narratives of daily

activities created by the researcher for rating purposes;

summary narratives were created in the current study of

10 individuals (Gioia & Brekke, 2009). After the initial

coding was completed by the author, instructions were

given to a second coder to make judgments about the fit

or lack of fit with the behavioral styles mentioned above.

This analytic task effectively quantitized the qualitative

data into categories that could be compared across coders.

Gioia 1501

Using this approach, Gioia and Brekke (2009) found that

having two coders independently make inferences from

the text was a useful approach for achieving credibility of

the data. Cohen’s (1960) Kappa is a statistical formula

that takes into account the relationship between the proportion of times when the raters agree on the data and the

proportion of times when agreement might have been

expected by chance. The level of agreement in this study

was .77, which meets the benchmark for achieving a substantial level of agreement between coders. Future

iterations of this model should be able to incorporate

multiple coders making independent ratings of the field

notes from a greater number of study participants on

these and other dimensions of interest to the study. It

might be possible to include the mental health clinician

who works with the individual to verify the predominant

behavioral style. This was not included in this study but

would provide an excellent additional means of study

rigor in future research. This level of analysis is complex,

and the relationship between neurocognition and behavior in the open environment can be moderated by many

contextual variables, including other physical or mental

illnesses, which will need to be considered from the qualitative findings in the definition of cognitive impairment

(Franzen & Wilhelm, 1996).

Step 8: Using the Cocreated Results

Results should optimally be cocreated and checked with

the participant living in the community. In other words,

validating that the strategy used to offset the neurocognitive

deficit was one that was agreed on by both the researcher

and the participant. Fully employing a participatory

action research (PAR) model into the eight steps would

render the findings clinically useful as a means to educate,

empower, and engage the individual, their support

network, the researcher, and the research audience about

the challenges to perform daily tasks in the community

when one has a disorder that affects cognition (Kidd &

Kral, 2005). Persons with neurocognitive impairment are

typically cut off from PAR engagement because of stigma

and other beliefs about their ability to participate.

However, this research method of ecological validity

could be ideal for consciousness raising for stakeholders,

and contributes an essential understanding of the notions

of ecological validity in these NP disorders.

Discussion

Although there has been a tendency to focus on negative

aspects of cognitive deficits that result from injuries and

illnesses, we know that some individuals with NP deficits

do not necessarily remain at an impaired level when they

are in community settings (Gioia, 2006; Koren et al,

2006). Why this happens for some and not for others is at

the heart of an ecological validity method. A step-bystep process was outlined to operationalize a method for

enhancing ecological validity by utilizing communitybased observation methods to assess whether there was

any correlation between NP functioning in the laboratory

and real-world functioning. Although eight steps have

been described, there is likely a way to tailor this approach

for use in a variety of projects. Step 8 is recommended but

could be omitted. The other steps are optimal for producing data on ecological validity. The proposed model of

behavioral community observation of cognition and tasks

has been suggested as a next step by many researchers.

The eight-step method appears to be relatively

straightforward, with the exception of the prolonged time

commitment, but it is deceptively complex, and presents

difficulties that are inherent to the fieldwork process and

the population being studied. It also might be difficult

because of the culture, gender, and ages of the participant

and the field researcher. Some of the eight steps might be

problematic for individuals with schizophrenia, and in

fact, in this sample, 10 individuals who had paucity of

speech or ongoing psychotic symptoms required accommodations to the schedule. One individual who became

paranoid during our research time in the community

required shortening the session by one day, but we were

able to resume at a later time. Research decisions might

have to be made to maximize the ability of the participant

to comfortably engage in the process, and yet the reality

of a prolonged engagement is likely to tire individuals

and risk the possibility of prematurely ending the participant observation. It might also be embarrassing for the

individual to have someone accompany her or him everywhere for a few days, or there might be times when safety

in the community is a concern for the researcher and the

participant. These potential community interactions can

be viewed as data about the process of obtaining ecological validity, and will certainly assist in refining the steps

of the proposed method.

Cognitive difficulties are a significant factor in many

chronic mental and physical conditions, such as Alzheimer’s disease, multiple sclerosis, schizophrenia, Parkinson’s

disease, cardiac conditions, traumatic brain injury, substance use, and normal aging. A limitation of this and

other NP studies is that there are often multiple contributions to cognitive decline which cannot really be separated

at the time of the data collection (e.g., silent heart disease). Most neuropsychologists continue with their studies

despite this real-world messiness that creates interpretation difficulties. It is hoped that this proposed model of

measuring ecological validity might be used across populations, and might provide a more attenuated description

1502 Qualitative Health Research 19(10)

of disparate capacities within and between individuals

with the same disorders. It is important that future

researchers exploring NP consider not only laboratory or

proxy findings, but provide a view of the individual as he

or she functions in his or her own environment. Even

though this initial study was mainly concerned with the

functional aspect of ecological validity—and not the predictive capacity—refinement of this method with more

participants might yield a greater ability to predict how

well individuals will be able to attend to routine and nonroutine tasks with and without social assistance.

The triangulation of observational and NP test data

should shed light on the differences in and types of cognitive decline, as well as differences in and types of

strategies across populations and illnesses. It might tease

out the question of whether the same neuropsychological

test is as relevant for someone with schizophrenia as it is

for someone with traumatic brain injury. In other words,

are designations of high and low functioning from the lab

results telling us the same thing, or do specific illness

characteristics play a larger part in the outcome? Individuals collecting the observational data would need

specific procedural training in field methods to ensure

that probes concerning the tasks are similar across field

researchers.

One possibility for a future study would be to have

four types of data collected for each individual within a

single study: (a) scores from NP laboratory measures; (b) a

proxy measure such as self-report, use of PDAs, or performance in one of the coprimary measures of NP (Green

et al., 2008); (c) significant other/mental health professional reports; and (d) the prolonged participant field

observation described here. Although this level of analysis will have complex challenges, it is promising to be

able to utilize all known methods for understanding NP

capacity and thus increase the potential for predictions of

community functioning and subsequent matching with

treatment interventions. It is hoped that this information

will contribute to efforts aimed at restoring or preserving

individual cognitive functioning. Cognitive remediation

is a current area of research that uses an array of behavioral interventions, mainly computer programs and other

targeted learning tools, to enable individuals to retrieve

some of their lost cognitive skills by teaching a variety of

compensatory strategies (Medalia & Richardson, 2005).

Four additional considerations need to become part of

the evaluation of an ecological validity model in the

future: (a) Can the steps of the model be accomplished in

a timely manner and still produce the complexity of data

necessary for a rigorous analysis?; (b) Can this ecological

validity approach be used to map the actual daily performance of slightly larger samples (n = 25), or subsamples

of much larger studies?; (c) What is the cost/benefit of

this ecological validity approach as compared to other

means of collecting information on NP functioning, and

what does it add?; and (d) Will this data be used to

develop interventions that benefit the recovery of the

individual in the community?

Author’s Note

I extend special thanks to the study participants for allowing me

to be part of their daily lives in the community, and to Dr. John

Brekke for his research guidance and mentorship.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the

authorship and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author disclosed receipt of the following financial support

for the research and/or authorship of this article: This research

was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R03

MH64686-01A1).

References

Bellack, A. S., Green, M. F., Cook, J. A., Fenton, W., Harvey,

P. D., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2007). Assessment of community functioning in people with schizophrenia and

other severe mental illnesses: A white paper based on a

National Institute of Mental Health sponsored workshop.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 805-822.

Brekke, J. S., Ell, K., Palinkas, L. A. (2007). Translational science at the National Institute of Mental Health: Can social

work take its rightful place? Research on Social Work Practice,

17(1), 123-133.

Brekke, J. S., Raine, A., Ansel, M., Lencz, T., & Bird, L. (1997).

Neuropsychological and psychophysiological correlates of

psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia

Bulletin, 23(1), 19-28.

Brown, C. E., Rempfer, M. V., Hamera, E., & Bothwell, R.

(2006). Knowledge of grocery shopping skills as a mediator

of cognition and performance. Psychiatric Services, 57(4),

573-575.

Carlsson, E., Paterson, B. L., Scott-Findlay, S., Ehnfors, M. &

Ehrenberg, A. (2007). Methodological issues involving people with communication impairments after acquired brain

damage. Qualitative Health Research, 17, 1361-1371.

Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2003). The ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: A review of the

literature on everyday cognitive skills. Neuropsychology

Review, 13(4), 181-197.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales.

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37-46.

Creswell, J. W., & Plano Clark, V. L. (2007). Designing and

conducting mixed methods research. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

Gioia 1503

Franzen, M. D., & Wilhelm, K. L. (1996). Conceptual foundations of ecological validity in neuropsychological assessment. In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Long (Eds.), Ecological

validity of neuropsychological testing. Delray Beach, FL:

GR Press/St Lucie Press.

Gioia, D. (2006). A contextual study of daily living strategies

in neurocognitively-impaired adults with schizophrenia.

Qualitative Health Research, 16, 1217-1235.

Gioia, D., & Brekke, J. S. (2009). Neurocognition, ecological

validity, and daily living in the community for individuals

with schizophrenia: A mixed methods study. Psychiatry:

Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 72(1), 93-106.

Granholm, E., Loh, C., & Swendson, J. (2008). Feasibility

and validity of computerized ecological momentary assessment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(3), 507-514.

Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., Kern, R. S., Baade, L. E., Fenton, W. S., Gold, J. M., et al. (2008). Functional co-primary

measures for clinical trials in schizophrenia: Results from the

Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition

in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) psychometric and standardization study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 221-228.

Greenwood, K. F., Landau, S., & Wykes, T. (2005). Negative

symptoms and specific cognitive impairments as combined

targets for improved functional outcome with cognitive

remediation therapy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(4), 910-921.

Groth-Marnat, G. (2000). Visions of clinical assessment: Then,

now, and a brief history of the future. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 56(3), 349-365.

Hammersley, M., & Atkinson, P. (1995). Ethnography: Principles in practice. New York: Routledge.

Kidd, S. A., & Kral, M. J. (2005). Practicing participatory action

research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 187-195.

Koren, D., Seidman, L. J., Goldsmith, M., & Harvey,

P. D. (2006). Real-world cognitive- and metacognitivedysfunction: A new approach for measuring and remediating more “right stuff.” Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 310-326.

Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older

people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily

living. Gerontologist, 9, 179-186.

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Johannesen, J. K.,

Nicolo, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2005). Metacognition amidst

narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: Associations

with neurocognition, symptoms, insight and quality of life.

Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112(1), 64-71.

Medalia, A., & Richardson, R. (2005). What predicts a good

response to cognitive remediation interventions? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(4), 942-953.

Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P. H., & Van Os, J. (2005).

Behavioral sensitization to daily life stress in psychosis.

Psychological Medicine, 35(5), 733-741.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2008). National Institute of Mental Health strategic plan (NIH Publication No.

08-6368). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

Retrieved August 15, 2009, from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/

about/strategic-planning-reports/index.shtml.

Patterson, T. L., Goldman, S., McKibbin, C. L., Hughs, T.,

& Jeste, D. V. (2001). UCSD performance-based skills

assessment: Development of a new measure of everyday

functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophrenia

Bulletin, 27(2), 235-245.

Randolph, E. T. (1998). Social networks and schizophrenia. In

K. T. Mueser & N. Tarrier (Eds.). Handbook of social functioning in schizophrenia (pp. 238-246). Needham Heights,

MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Rempfer, M. V., Hamera, E. K., Brown, C. E., & Cromwell, R. L.

(2003). The relationship between cognition and the independent living skill of shopping in people with schizophrenia.

Psychiatry Research, 117(2), 103-112.

Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T. &

Yiend, J. (1997). Oops!: Performance correlated of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747-758.

Sbordone, R. J (1996). Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing. In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Long (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing (pp. 15-42).

Delray Beach, FL: GR Press/St Lucie Press.

Scrimshaw, S. C. M., Carballo, M., Ramos, L., & Blair, B. A.

(1991). The AIDS rapid anthropological assessment procedures: A tool for health education and planning. Health Education & Behavior, 18(1), 111-123.

Tierney, M. C., Snow, W. G., Charles, J., Moineddin, R., &

Kiss, A. (2007). Neuropsychological predictors of selfneglect in cognitively-impaired older people who live

alone. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(2),

140-148.

van Manen, J. V. (1988). Tales of the field: On writing ethnography. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Willis, G. B. (2005). Cognitive interviewing: A tool for improving questionnaire design. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Wolcott, H. F. (1995). The art of fieldwork. Lanham, MD:

AltaMira Press.

Bio

Deborah Gioia, PhD, LCSW, is an associate professor at the

University of Maryland, Baltimore, School of Social Work,

Baltimore, Maryland, USA.